27.49
***p < 0.001.
The mean differences of the HCBS between the groups of grades.
To address the gap in the previous research on homework creativity, this study examined the psychometric proprieties of the HCBS and its relationship with academic achievement and general creativity. The main findings were (1) Hypotheses H1a and H1b were supported that the reliability and validity of the HCBS were acceptable; (2) Hypothesis H2 was supported that the correlation between the score of the HCBS and academic achievement was significant ( r -values = 0.23–0.26 for two samples); (3) Hypothesis H3 received support that the correlation between the scores of HCBS and WCAP was significant ( r -values = 0.20–0.29 for two samples); and (4) the H4 was supported from the current data that the score of high school students’ was lower than that of the middle school students’ (Cohen’s d = 0.49).
The first key finding should be noted is that the positive correlations with between pairs of homework creativity, homework completion, and general creativity. This result is inconsistent with prediction of an argument that homework diminishes creativity ( Cooper et al., 2012 ; Zheng, 2013 ). Specifically, the correlation between homework completion and curiosity was insignificant ( r = 0.08, p > 0.05) which did not support the argument that homework hurts curiosity of creativity ( Zheng, 2013 ). The possible reason may be homework can provide opportunities to foster some components of creativity by independently finding and developing new ways of understanding what students have learned in class, as Kaiipob (1951) argued. It may be the homework creativity that served as the way to practice the components of general creativity. In fact, the content of items of the HCBS are highly related with creative thinking (refer to Table 2 for details).
The second key finding should be noted is that the score of the HCBS decreased as the level of grades increased from 7 to 11. This is consistent with the basic trend recorded in the previous meta-analyses ( Kim, 2011 ; Said-Metwaly et al., 2021 ). There are three possible explanations leading to this grade effect. The first one is the repetitive exercises in homework. As Zheng (2013) observed, to get higher scores in the highly competitive entrance examination of high school and college, those Chinese students chose to practice a lot of repetitive exercises. The results of some behavior experiments suggested that repetitive activity could reduce the diverse thinking of subjects’ (e.g., Main et al., 2020 ). Furthermore, the repetitive exercises would lead to fast habituation (can be observed by skin conductance records) which hurts the creative thinking of participants ( Martindale et al., 1996 ). The second explanation is that the stress level in Chinese high schools is higher than in middle school because of the college entrance examination. The previous studies (e.g., Beversdorf, 2018 ) indicated that the high level of stress will trigger the increase activity of the noradrenergic system and the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis which could debase the individual’s performance of creativity. Another likely explanation is the degree of the certainty of the college entrance examination. The level of certainty highly increases (success or failure) when time comes closer to the deadline of the entrance examination. The increase of degree of certainty will lead to the decrease of activity of the brain areas related to curiosity (e.g., Jepma et al., 2012 ).
From the theoretical perspective, there are two points deserving to be emphasized. First, the findings of this study extended the previous work ( Beghetto and Kaufman, 2007 ; Kaufman and Beghetto, 2009 ). This study revealed that homework creativity had two typical characteristics, including the personal meaning of students (as represented by the content of items of the HCBS) and the small size of “creativity” and limited in the scope of exercises (small correlations with general creativity). These characteristics are in line with what Mini-C described by the previous studies ( Beghetto and Kaufman, 2007 ; Kaufman and Beghetto, 2009 ). Second, this study deepened our understanding of the relationship between learning (homework is a part of learning) and creativity which has been discussed more than half a century. One of the main viewpoints is learning and creativity share some fundamental similarities, but no one explained what is the content of these “fundamental similarities” (e.g., Gajda et al., 2017 ). This study identified one similarity between learning and creativity in the context of homework, that is homework creativity. Homework creativity has the characteristics of homework and creativity at the same time which served as an inner factor in which homework promote creativity.
The findings in this study also have several potential practical implications. First, homework creativity should be a valuable goal of learning, because homework creativity may make contributions to academic achievement and general creativity simultaneously. They accounted for a total of 10.7% variance of academic achievement and general creativity which are the main goals of learning. Therefore, it is valuable to imbed homework creativity as a goal of learning, especially in the Chinese society ( Zheng, 2013 ).
Second, the items of the HCBS can be used as a vehicle to help students how to develop about homework creativity. Some studies indicated that the creative performance of students will improve just only under the simple requirement of “to be creative please” ( Niu and Sternberg, 2003 ). Similarly, some simple requirements, like “to do your homework in an innovative way,” “don’t stick to what you learned in class,” “to use a simpler method to do your homework,” “to use your imagination when you do homework,” “to design new problems on the basis what learnt,” “to find your own unique insights into your homework,” and “to find multiple solutions to the problem,” which rewritten from the items of the HCBS, can be used in the process of directing homework of students. In fact, these directions are typical behaviors of creative teaching (e.g., Soh, 2000 ); therefore, they are highly possible to be effective.
Third, the HCBS can be used to measure the degree of homework creativity in ordinary teaching or experimental situations. As demonstrated in the previous sections, the reliability and validity of the HCBS were good enough to play such a role. Based on this tool, the educators can collect the data of homework creativity, and make scientific decisions to improve the performance of people’s teaching or learning.
The main contribution is that this study accumulated some empirical knowledge about the relationship among homework creativity, homework completion, academic achievement, and general creativity, as well as the psychometric quality of the HCBS. However, the findings of this study should be treated with cautions because of the following limitations. First, our study did not collect the test–retest reliability of the HCBS. This makes it difficult for us to judge the HCBS’s stability over time. Second, the academic achievement data in our study were recorded by self-reported methods, and the objectivity may be more accurate. Third, the lower reliability coefficients existed in two dimensions employed, i.e., the arrange environment of the HMS (the α coefficient was 0.63), and the adventure of the WCAP (the α coefficient was 0.61). Fourth, the samples included here was not representative enough if we plan to generalize the finding to the population of middle and high school students in main land of China.
In addition to those questions listed as laminations, there are a number of issues deserve further examinations. (1) Can these findings from this study be generalized into other samples, especially into those from other cultures? For instances, can the reliability and validity of the HCBS be supported by the data from other samples? Or can the grade effect of the score of the HCBS be observed in other societies? Or can the correlation pattern among homework creativity, homework completion, and academic achievement be reproduced in other samples? (2) What is the role of homework creativity in the development of general creativity? Through longitudinal study, we can systematically observe the effect of homework creativity on individual’s general creativity, including creative skills, knowledge, and motivation. The micro-generating method ( Kupers et al., 2018 ) may be used to reveal how the homework creativity occurs in the learning process. (3) What factors affect homework creativity? Specifically, what effects do the individual factors (e.g., gender) and environmental factors (such as teaching styles of teachers) play in the development of homework creativity? (4) What training programs can be designed to improve homework creativity? What should these programs content? How about their effect on the development of homework creativity? What should the teachers do, if they want to promote creativity in their work situation? All those questions call for further explorations.
Homework is a complex thing which might have many aspects. Among them, homework creativity was the latest one being named ( Guo and Fan, 2018 ). Based on the testing of its reliability and validity, this study explored the relationships between homework creativity and academic achievement and general creativity, and its variation among different grade levels. The main findings of this study were (1) the eight-item version of the HCBS has good validity and reliability which can be employed in the further studies; (2) homework creativity had positive correlations with academic achievement and general creativity; (3) compared with homework completion, homework creativity made greater contribution to general creativity, but less to academic achievement; and (4) the score of homework creativity of high school students was lower than that of middle school students. Given that this is the first investigation, to our knowledge, that has systematically tapped into homework creativity, there is a critical need to pursue this line of investigation further.
Ethics statement.
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the research ethic committee, School of Educational Science, Bohai University. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.
HF designed the research, collected the data, and interpreted the results. YM and SG analyzed the data and wrote the manuscript. HF, JX, and YM revised the manuscript. YC and HF prepared the HCBS. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
We thank Dr. Liwei Zhang for his supports in collecting data, and Lu Qiao, Dounan Lu, Xiao Zhang for their helps in the process of inputting data.
This work was supported by the LiaoNing Revitalization Talents Program (grant no. XLYC2007134) and the Funding for Teaching Leader of Bohai University.
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.923882/full#supplementary-material
The study, led by professor Harris Cooper, also shows that the positive correlation is much stronger for secondary students than elementary students
It turns out that parents are right to nag: To succeed in school, kids should do their homework.
Duke University researchers have reviewed more than 60 research studies on homework between 1987 and 2003 and concluded that homework does have a positive effect on student achievement.
Harris Cooper, a professor of psychology, said the research synthesis that he led showed the positive correlation was much stronger for secondary students --- those in grades 7 through 12 --- than those in elementary school.
READ MORE: Harris Cooper offers tips for teaching children in the next school year in this USA Today op-ed published Monday.
"With only rare exception, the relationship between the amount of homework students do and their achievement outcomes was found to be positive and statistically significant," the researchers report in a paper that appears in the spring 2006 edition of "Review of Educational Research."
Cooper is the lead author; Jorgianne Civey Robinson, a Ph.D. student in psychology, and Erika Patall, a graduate student in psychology, are co-authors. The research was supported by a grant from the U.S. Department of Education.
While it's clear that homework is a critical part of the learning process, Cooper said the analysis also showed that too much homework can be counter-productive for students at all levels.
"Even for high school students, overloading them with homework is not associated with higher grades," Cooper said.
Cooper said the research is consistent with the "10-minute rule" suggesting the optimum amount of homework that teachers ought to assign. The "10-minute rule," Cooper said, is a commonly accepted practice in which teachers add 10 minutes of homework as students progress one grade. In other words, a fourth-grader would be assigned 40 minutes of homework a night, while a high school senior would be assigned about two hours. For upper high school students, after about two hours' worth, more homework was not associated with higher achievement.
The authors suggest a number of reasons why older students benefit more from homework than younger students. First, the authors note, younger children are less able than older children to tune out distractions in their environment. Younger children also have less effective study habits.
But the reason also could have to do with why elementary teachers assign homework. Perhaps it is used more often to help young students develop better time management and study skills, not to immediately affect their achievement in particular subject areas.
"Kids burn out," Cooper said. "The bottom line really is all kids should be doing homework, but the amount and type should vary according to their developmental level and home circumstances. Homework for young students should be short, lead to success without much struggle, occasionally involve parents and, when possible, use out-of-school activities that kids enjoy, such as their sports teams or high-interest reading."
Cooper pointed out that there are limitations to current research on homework. For instance, little research has been done to assess whether a student's race, socioeconomic status or ability level affects the importance of homework in his or her achievement.
This is Cooper's second synthesis of homework research. His first was published in 1989 and covered nearly 120 studies in the 20 years before 1987. Cooper's recent paper reconfirms many of the findings from the earlier study.
Cooper is the author of "The Battle over Homework: Common Ground for Administrators, Teachers, and Parents" (Corwin Press, 2001).
Copy and paste the URL below to share this page.
Middle school students' perceptions regarding the motivation and effectiveness of homework., phenomenological study of middle school teacher practices regarding homework in an eastern north carolina rural community, 95 references, homework practices and academic achievement: the mediating role of self-efficacy and perceived responsibility beliefs, classwork and homework in early adolescence: the ecology of achievement.
Looking at homework differently, meanings of homework and implications for practice, the forgotten voices in homework: views of students, does homework improve academic achievement a synthesis of research, 1987–2003, homework and attainment in primary schools, parental involvement, homework, and tv time: direct and indirect effects on high school achievement, homework as the job of childhood, related papers.
Showing 1 through 3 of 0 Related Papers
The course website and blog for the fall 2015 instance of penn state's sc200 course.
We all hate homework. It’s tedious, frustrating, time-consuming, and downright horrible. Sometimes we get points for doing homework and doing well which is always a good reason for getting it done, but could success on homework be the reason for fantastic final grades?
Let’s establish the basics of what we are trying to find here. The x-variable is doing your homework while the y-variable is earning excellent grades. Confounding, z-variables , could include personality traits, lack of procrastination habits, natural ability to succeed in school, etc. Our null hypothesis is that doing your homework does not improve your final grade . Our alternative hypothesis is that doing your homework does improve your final grade and promotes academic achievement.
Harris Cooper , a professor of psychology and neuroscience at Duke University, and his colleagues compiled an analysis of dozens of studies done on homework in order to come to a conclusion on whether homework is effective. If it is effective, how much homework is too much, and what is the appropriate amount to give out to students?
Many of the studies done on this question examine students who are assigned homework with students who are not assigned homework but are still similar in other ways. Interestingly, many of the results found that homework can improve test scores at the end of a topic. “Students assigned homework in 2nd grade did better on math, 3rd and 4th graders did better on English skills and vocabulary, 5th graders on social studies, 9th through 12th graders on American history, and 12th graders on Shakespeare.” ( Cooper )
Some studies do not attempt to control for student differences. 35 studies suggest that 77% find the correlation between homework and and academic achievement to be positive; however, they fail to make this correlation among elementary students. One possible solution to control for student differences would be to randomly distribute the students based on similarities so that on average, both the homework group and the non-homework group are about the same in terms of similarities, i.e. learning disabilities, gender, and prior achievement in school. Additionally, Cooper says an explanation for why there is not a correlation among elementary students could be because they do not have well developed study habits and because they get distracted easily.
In short, Cooper suggests that through his analysis, homework is in fact beneficial to students . Not only can it have positive effects on overall grades, but it can also have other benefits such as developing responsible character traits, maturing cognitive capacities, fostering independent learning habits, and growing of good study habits. Cooper, along with most educators, says homework should not exceed 10-20 minutes for children K-2, 30-60 minutes a day for grades 3-6, and varying times depending on the subjects for middle school and high school students.
Some feel that homework can have many negative effects such as developing a disinterest in school among students, homework denies children of leisure time and takes them away from extra-curricular activities which also teach important life skills. It is important to allow teachers and administrators to have flexibility to account for the differences in some students and their families; however, sticking to the prescribed regiment is most effective for most students.
Rival ACC school, the University of Virginia, has a much different take on homework than Cooper. Co-authors Adam Maltese, assistant professor of science education at Indiana University, Robert H. Tai, associate professor of science education at the University of Virginia’s Curry School of Education, and Xitao Fan, dean of education at the University of Macau, conducted their own studies and published “ When Is Homework Worth the Time ?”
Because the paper is twenty-two pages long, I will summarize the findings. If you would like to, the full report can be read here . 18,000, tenth grade students’s survey and transcript data were observed in the study collected from 1990 to 2002 by the National Center for Education Statistics . Unlike many studies done on homework and final grades, Maltese, Tai, and Fan found that time spent on homework did not effect the final course grade among those who did and did not do their homework. Conversely, they did find a correlation between time spent on homework and success on standardized test scores. Maltese says, “Our results hint that maybe homework is not being used as well as it could be.” In order to be more effective with homework, teachers should assign homework which is useful, sort of a quality over quantity type of thing. Rather than give a designated amount of homework, give assignments which will keep the students engaged for a short period of time and allow for a greater chance of retaining that information. In effect, this will also allow for appropriate amounts of time to be allocated towards extracurricular activities which teach young people other valuable lessons while also learning from engaging homework.
All of this raises the question: what is the most effective type of homework assignment? I certainly feel as though this question can best be answered based on each individual person. Because some people are inherently auditory, visual, or hands-on learners, one standard type of homework cannot be called the best . I believe in order to really get the best result from everyone, each person would require their own homework regiment. Seeing as though some schools have entire graduating classes of well-over 2,000 students , creating an individualized homework regiment for each student is simply impossible. So what basic principles should teachers and administrators use to create effect homework?
The Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development ( ASCD ) attempted to tackle this tricky question with their “ Five Hallmarks of Good Homework .” The first principle is purpose . Students must be given a clear end goal to their assignment such as giving simple division problems in order to understand the concept of division or writing sentences using certain vocabulary words so that students can understand the context of those specific vocabulary words. In addition, ASCD says practice is most effective when given in small doses over long periods of time, concurrent with Maltese, Tai, and Fan. The second principle is efficiency . ASCD says projects which involve cutting, gluing, and constructing are often extremely inefficient even though the teacher has great intentions when they assign them because they are fun and creative. Instead, rather than making a poster, students should be tasked to put themselves in the perspective of their project. For example, ASCD suggests if students are tasked with a history assignment, they should be asked to create a diary entry as if they were the person who experienced what they are trying to learn (writing about what it was like to immigrate from another country, writing about what World War 2 was like, etc.). The third principle is ownership . One of the easiest ways to promote ownership is by giving flexibility. Instead of prescribing a common book for the class to read, teachers could allow students to find their own sources such as magazines and academic journals which are still relevant to the topic. This keeps the students engaged and interested in what they are learning. “Instead of worrying about whether students did the reading, we should be focusing on whether the reading did them any good” ( ASCD ). The fourth principle is competence . Because, each student is different, they should be allowed to work together if they choose to and receive help on assignments. Students often get discouraged when forced to work alone and are more likely not to complete a task. The fifth, and final, principle is aesthetic appeal . First impressions are extremely important to students. As soon as they see the requirements and details of an assignment, they make a snap decision about whether they are going to do it or not and, if they are going to do it, how well they are going to do it. Students are more inclined to complete an assignment which are visually uncluttered with few information on the page. Lots of room to write answers and the use of graphics and clip art on the page are also quite appealing to students. Visuals are just as important to the student as knowing they have little work to do.
Take home message: homework is beneficial to the student in more ways than just improving final grades but only when allocated effectively . In my opinion, and I think most would agree, there need to be more studies done on the effectiveness of homework. Preferably, some kind of experimental study would be conducted to almost definitively prove that effective homework benefits the student in multiple ways. Of course, a double-blind placebo would be out of the question because the student would know if they are doing their homework or not. Maybe a single-blind study could be effective where the students are randomly placed into two groups, homework and no homework. The teacher would not know who is and who is not doing their homework, but would still assign regular assignments to the class. The students either do or do not complete their homework, and at the end of the semester or grading period, examine the results of how many students received good or bad marks on their final reports. Of course, this study would flawed in that if a student gets placed into the group who does not do their homework but normally would have done their homework and their grade suffers from not doing it, that is infringing on the student’s ability and right to learn, and compromises their own responsibility for their grades; however, at this point, this is the closest I could get to an appropriate experimental study. Any other suggestions would be greatly appreciated in the comments.
Picture Links:
http://cdn.kidspot.com.au/wp-content/uploads/2013/02/hateshomework-600×420.jpg
http://familyfieldguide.com/wp-content/uploads/2013/08/boy-doing-homework.jpg
http://www.primaryteaching.co.uk/prodimg/B4_1_Zoom.jpg
Very interesting post. In high school, I was adamantly anti-homework, generally equating homework with meaningless busywork, but your post got me thinking, and I came up with an idea for a study that sort of expands on your idea. Here’s what I’ve got: a class made specially for the study is split into two groups via random assignment, and one group is assigned homework while the other is told not to do the homework. The study will include a random sample of students of the same grade level or year, and everyone will come to class as required and will be encouraged to be active in the classroom and really pay attention to what is being taught (the teacher will find a way to get around to this somehow). Besides the homework, the only real assignments given are in-class quizzes and a final at the end of the semester, which is when the grades of those who did and didn’t do the homework will be compared. The homework, of course, will be GOOD homework, as determined by the five hallmarks you went over in your post. Also, this class will institute a NO-STUDY policy. That’s important. It will be physically impossible to study for the tests anyway, because there is nothing that students can read or study from at home — no handouts, nothing. (The entire curriculum may as well be completely fabricated.) This study is far from a perfect setup and I’m sure it contains some major flaws in reasoning, the most obvious of which is the question of the students’ drive and motivation to actually try on the homework in the first place (since this won’t be a class that they’re technically graded on, so it may not be a true measure of their aptitude and ability… but that’s still better than the alternative). Anyhow, I could see it turning up some interesting results. Given that the homework demonstrates the strongest possible examples of the five hallmarks of good homework, and the students assigned homework put forth their best effort on the homework assignments, I think that the homework-assigned group could receive better overall grades than the no-homework group.
Comments are closed.
Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.
To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to upgrade your browser .
Enter the email address you signed up with and we'll email you a reset link.
Download Free PDF
This study takes advantage of nationally representative panel data on student behavior and academic performance to test two possible policy reforms. First, I examine a policy that increases the amount of homework that students complete. Second, I examine the impact of increasing the amount of homework assigned. Previous studies have not been able to consistently estimate the impact of homework because of important omitted variables and measurement error, which strongly bias the estimated impact of homework time. This paper, however, uses an instrumental variables approach with student fixed effects to account for both time-varying and time-invariant unobserved characteristics and inputs. This approach produces estimates of the impact of homework time on academic achievement that are much larger than those of previous studies. Additionally, these findings suggest that assigning additional homework primarily improves the achievement of low performing students and students in low performing schools. Thus, assigning more homework could help close the gap in achievement between high and low performing students.
In the literature on the impact of homework there is little empirical support for assigning homework to elementary school students. Nevertheless, the practice has become more common, despite popular resistance among many parents and popular media. We examine the effects of both assigning homework and time spent on homework on mathematics and reading achievement using nationally representative longitudinal data on elementary school students. In order to control for important unobserved characteristics and inputs we use empirical specifications that include student fixed effects. We find that this approach consistently indicates that homework has a positive impact on academic achievement, and that less sophisticated empirical approaches will produce misleading results. Additionally, we find that the impact of homework is not uniform across the population, but that some minority groups and low income students get more benefit from homework, indicating that increasing homework assigned could be a valuable policy for decreasing the black-white as well as the high and low-income achievement gap.
Journal of Labor Research, 2011
School Psychology Quarterly, 2004
Children’s Homework Time—Do Parents ’ Investments Make a Difference? This article describes the homework time of 2024 children in school grades 1 through 12, using time diary data from a national dataset, the Panel Study of Income Dynamics-Child Development Supplement (PSID-CDS). As part of the PSID-CDS, time diary data were collected for one randomly selected weekday and weekend day. Data were analyzed with an investment model perspective, where parental time, money, and human capital were expected to influence children’s and adolescents ’ homework time. About 2/3 of children did any homework. Logistic regression revealed that ethnicity was the primary predictor of whether or not children in elementary school and junior high school did any homework, although some investment model variables, in particular time (number of children in household) was significant for elementary school children, and money (family income) was influential for junior high school students. All three investme...
Education and Urban Society, 2015
As schools aim to raise student academic achievement levels and districts wrangle with decreased funding, it is essential to understand the relationship between learning time and academic achievement. Using regression analysis and a data set drawn from California’s elementary school sites, we find a statistically significant and positive relationship between the number of instructional minutes in an academic year and school-site standardized test scores. Fifteen more minutes of school a day at a school site (or about an additional week of classes over an academic year) relates to an increase in average overall academic achievement of about 1%, and about a 1.5% increase in average achievement for disadvantaged students. This same increase in learning time yields the much larger 37% gain in the average growth of socioeconomically disadvantage achievement from the previous academic year. Placing this impact in the context of other influences found important to academic achievement, similar increases in achievement only occur with an increase of fully credentialed teachers by nearly 7 percentage points. These findings offer guidance regarding the use of extended learning time to increase academic performance. Moreover, they suggest caution in reducing instructional time as the default approach to managing fiscal challenges.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 1986
The main purpose of this study was to determine the effect of homework assignments on students' academic achievement. This meta-analysis sought an answer to the research question: "What kind of effect does homework assignment have on students' academic achievement levels?" In this research, meta-analysis was adopted to determine the effect of homework assignments on students' academic achievement. The effect sizes of the studies included in the meta-analysis were compared with regard to their methodological characteristics (research design, sample size, and publication bias) and substantive characteristics (course type, grade level, duration of implementation, instructional level, socioeconomic status, and setting). At the end of the research, it was revealed that homework assignments had a small effect size (d = 0.229) on students' academic achievement levels. Lastly, it was seen that there was not a significant difference with regard to the effect sizes of the studies with respect to all variables, except the course type variable in the research.
Loading Preview
Sorry, preview is currently unavailable. You can download the paper by clicking the button above.
Theory Into Practice, 2004
Economics of Education Review, 1987
PROSPECTS, 2004
Frontiers in Psychology, 2019
Social Psychology of Education, 1999
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 2002
International Journal of Behavioral Development, 2013
Http Dx Doi Org 10 1080 09645292 2011 585794, 2011
Frontiers in Psychology, 2017
Educational Psychology, 2014
Psicología Educativa: Revista de los Psicólogos de la Educación, 2021
BMC Psychology volume 12 , Article number: 498 ( 2024 ) Cite this article
Metrics details
While previous research has identified executive functions as predictors of academic performance in school children, similar studies conducted among adults show mixed results. One of the reasons given for executive functions having a limited effect on academic achievements in adulthood is that they are usually fully developed by that time. Since these executive functions are at their peak at that age, the individual differences in these as well as their influence on academic performance in adults are harder to trace. The paper describes a study conducted among 107 university students the goal of which was to find out whether there is any relationship between the adult students’ inhibitory control values measured with the Stroop Test and their academic achievements. Although the results indicate a weak correlation between the Stroop Effect and the students’ academic performance of low statistical significance, which seems to confirm the outcomes of the previous studies focusing on adults, the study reveals an unexpected statistically significant correlation between the students’ grade averages and the number of their incorrect color identifications. This phenomenon appears to be worth pursuing in future research since it suggests the existence of another, relatively quickly measurable, variable possibly reflecting other predictors of academic performance in adults such as a degree of their manifested conscientiousness, their ability to concentrate on an assigned, relatively short, one-off task and their attitude to fulfilling this task. The Stroop Test, despite not being originally designed for this purpose, might thus be used as a simple tool suitable for providing information about these variables via the subject’s number of color identification errors. Such information can subsequently inform the activities that educators may include in their curricula to foster conscientiousness and concentration in the students lacking these.
Peer Review reports
As documented by previous research, academic performance as the “level of knowledge demonstrated in an area or subject compared to the norm for the particular age and level of education” [ 46 ] has been affected by a myriad of factors. These are socioeconomic [ 66 ], student-related (e.g. students’ self-control and class attendance) [ 25 , 29 ], or psychosocial [ 60 ]. Another factor documented as affecting academic performance is a complex of executive functions which appear to play a significant role in language development as well as the processing and organization of received information [ 57 ]. The processing of received information is done either through automatic attention or controlled attention. In the case of the former, the attention responses direct attention automatically to a target regardless of concurrent inputs or memory load. In the case of the latter, an active attention of the subject is required, which also makes the information processing limited in terms of processing capacity [ 63 ].
Executive functions encompass cognitive skills related to attention control, i.e. the process by which attention is selectively directed to specific aspects of a representation, particularly in misleading situations [ 11 ]. One of the attention control mechanisms can be switching attention between tasks where, in the case of a card-sort test, for instance, the subject must switch between different rules by which they sort cards (e.g. first by their shape and then by their color). Another mechanism is the inhibition of attention when it comes to the stimuli that need to be ignored. This inhibition is activated in, for instance, multilinguals when they need to suppress their temptation to use one (or more) of their languages not needed or inappropriate for a given situation [ 11 , 13 , 77 , etc.]. Other researchers [ 2 , 53 , etc.] work with the terms of inhibition (the ability to suppress dominant responses, a term synonymous to the attention inhibition mentioned above), shifting (the ability to switch over between tasks, a term synonymous to switching attention mentioned above) and monitoring (the ability to update information in the working memory). The working memory as a function that enables individuals to temporarily remember information while competitively processing information [ 54 ] has been mentioned as a factor influencing school performance even more than intelligence with the latter predicting a wide range of indicators of academic success [ 48 , 52 ].
Multiple studies emphasize the fact that educational research should pay more attention to executive functions since these represent essential ingredients for successful academic functioning [ 75 ] and since they also appear to be connected with school dysfunction as deficits in them have been associated with disabilities in mathematics and reading [ 51 , 70 ]. According to Pascual [ 57 ], who also mentions cognitive flexibility, i.e. the ability to temporarily manipulate information, and planning, the executive functions represent “distinct, but related, higher-order neurocognitive processes that control thought and behaviors aimed at achieving an objective goal” (p. 2). The existence of relationship between executive functions and academic achievement is also supported by other studies most of which investigate this relationship in children of pre-school or early-school age (e.g. [ 4 , 9 , 19 , 28 , 76 , etc.]) or those that study it in the context of learning disabilities [ 3 , 37 , 49 , 64 ].
Some studies also stress the fact that the positive contribution of executive functions to academic performance is domain-dependent, i.e. that certain executive functions contribute to gaining certain knowledge or skill more than others. Thus, inhibition, for instance, appears to be beneficial when it comes to mathematics and science [ 57 ]. Similarly, Gerst [ 38 ] found a direct relationship between inhibition and the ability to conduct mathematical calculations in children aged 10–11.
As there seems to be a variation in the way in which younger and older children solve calculations due to the age-related shift from the procedural-based processing in arithmetic tasks to more memory-based [ 72 , 7 , 16 , etc.] it is also different types of interference that appear to disrupt children at different ages. In a dual-task study McKenzie et al. [ 50 ], for instance, found out that the mathematical processing of 6-year-old children was disrupted only by a visuo-spatial passive interference task whereas in the 8-year-old ones it was disrupted by both a visuo-spatial and a phonological interference task. In this respect, the type of information processing deployed in problem solving appears to determine the type of irrelevant stimuli that need to be suppressed through inhibition for the students to complete an arithmetic task efficiently.
The executive function of inhibition is usually defined as the ability to suppress dominant but irrelevant responses and prioritize important information instead. This way “it moderates behavior, suppresses impulsive reactions to a stimulus, and enables an appropriate and thoughtful response” [ 57 ]. Cognitive inhibition is thus responsible for planning, analyzing and choosing the most appropriate response.
The relationship between poor academic performance and poor performance in tasks requiring the inhibition of irrelevant information has been pointed out by multiple studies. Espy et al. [ 33 ], investigating how working memory and inhibitory control affect arithmetic competency, identified differences in the ability to inhibit irrelevant stimuli as a factor responsible for unique variance in mathematical skills. In mathematics, inhibitory control is used to inhibit information that should not be maintained in working memory for upcoming responding [ 27 ]. Similarly, Espy et al. point out that to flexibly shift responding in the face of conflicting rules requires maintaining the rule in mind and inhibiting prepotent, previous responses [ 33 ]. Passolunghi & Lanfranchi [ 55 ] mention inhibition as a factor influencing performance at the numerical competence test.
The importance of the role that inhibition plays in reading and listening comprehension has been pointed out by studies focusing on children. Passolunghi et al. [ 56 ], for instance, stress that groups of poor problem solvers tend to perform poorly in a working memory test requiring inhibition of irrelevant information and that this condition appears to be related to poor recall of critical information and greater recall of to-be-inhibited information. In addition, the process of reading often involves exposure to visual distractions such as images, graphs, etc. present in the very text as well as external physical distractors in the environment in which the reading takes place. In such situations, the inhibitory control helps the reader to stay focused on the written content. Similarly, De Beni et al. [ 26 ], showed that the “poor comprehenders” had a significantly lower performance in the listening span test associated with a higher number of intrusions. These intrusions can be background noise or competing sounds that need to be ignored for the listener to focus on and understand what a speaker is saying. In addition, both reading and listening often involve interpretation of figurative language, where the inhibition of the literal, irrelevant information enables the reader or listener to grasp the relevant meaning [ 39 ].
The positive correlation between a degree of inhibitory control and academic achievement has been documented by other studies as well. Duckworth et al. [ 29 ], for instance, stress behavioral inhibition (self-control) as affecting academic performance. St Clair-Thompson & Gathercole [ 62 ] identify inhibition as a factor associated with achievement in English, mathematics, and science in 11- and 12-year-old children while Blair & Razza [ 14 ] point out that the inhibitory control correlated with both early math and reading ability in their study conducted among 3- to 5-year-old children. Privitera et al. [ 59 ] give a reason for why the improved inhibitory control leads to greater academic performance; the students with improved inhibitory control can focus on tasks both within and outside of the classroom better, ignoring the ever-growing number of distractions present in their environments. The authors also claim that this improved focus may result in superior academic performance. Irvan & Tsapali [ 44 ] point out the positive effect of improved inhibitory control on academic performance stating that the inhibition as an executive function appears particularly crucial for young children growing up and learning as they are exposed to constant distractions vying for their attention. Blair & Razza [ 14 ] also suggest that curricula designed to improve self-regulation skills and enhance early academic abilities may be most effective in helping children succeed in school.
On the other hand, some sources conclude that the relationship between executive functions, with inhibitory control representing one of them, and academic performance appears to depend on age. Bryce et al. [ 15 ] focused on the relationship between executive functions and metacognitive skills, which they have identified as most significant predictors of educational achievements in their study groups of 5- and 7-year-old children. Their results indicate that executive functions appear to be more related to metacognitive skills in 5-year-olds than in 7-year-olds. In the study conducted among subjects aged 5–17, Best et al. [ 10 ] analyzed a varying correlation between executive functions and academic achievement in relation to age concluding that the correlation is strongest at the ages of 6, 8–9 and subsequently appears to be of somewhat consistent strength in the late childhood and adolescence. This conclusion partly contradicts the findings of Altemeier et al. [ 1 ], who claim that the effect of executive functions on academic performance may be more evident earlier in schooling, when academic skills are less automatic and require more effortful planning to execute. Similarly, some other studies point out inhibition as the strongest predictor of academic success at children’s early age such as that by Senn et al. [ 61 ]. They found out that while working memory contributed to academic success to a greater extent in older children, the inhibitory control did this in younger ones. Other authors [ 8 , 42 , etc.] mention the complete maturation of inhibitory processes by around the age of 12. The decrease in the potential of executive functions to predict academic performance during secondary education and even more so during university studies has also been touched upon by Pascual & Robres [ 57 ].
Scientists have developed several methods to measure inhibitory control, whose choice partly depends on the type of inhibition that is being targeted. Response inhibition, a term referring to the process of countermanding a prepotent motor response, has generally been assessed using non-selective stopping tasks such as the stop signal, go/no-go, and anti-saccade tasks. These tests require participants to intermittently suppress a motor response to a given presentation of a conditional stimulus or cue [ 6 , 20 , 71 , 73 ]. Attentional inhibition, which refers to the ability to resist interference from stimuli in the external environment, has been investigated using visual matching tasks requiring participants to judge whether target and comparison stimuli are the same or different and, at the same time, requiring them to ignore task-irrelevant distracters [ 36 , 68 , 71 ].
Response inhibition and attentional inhibition are also commonly measured with Stimulus-Response Compatibility tasks, such as the Eriksen Flanker (Flanker), Simon, and Stroop tasks [ 32 , 65 , 69 , 71 ]. The Stroop task (test) is utilized for comparing reaction times to stimuli in the condition where this control is not deployed (congruent condition) with the reaction times requiring inhibiting irrelevant stimuli (incongruent condition). The test has been shown to activate either the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex or anterior cingulate for cognitive inhibition [ 74 ]. As Imbrosciano & Berlach [ 43 ] point out, anterior cingulate is considered to be responsible for selecting an appropriate response when the brain is exposed to two conflicting conditions. Bush et al. [ 17 ] hypothesize that anterior cingulate dysfunction is responsible for producing core features of ADHD, namely inattention and impulsivity. Anterior cingulate activation has been linked to detection of conflict and its resolution [ 18 ] as well as to academic results in college students [ 40 ]. The last-named researchers investigated the activity of anterior cingulate in connection with the error-related negativity (ERN, an electrophysiological signal associated with the anterior cingulate monitoring process, occurring approximately 100 ms after an error is made) and found a correlation between the magnitude of ERN and undergraduate students’ academic performance suggesting that the error detection mechanism is stronger in the students who perform better at university. Veroude et al. [ 74 ] observed a positive correlation between average course grades and the activation of anterior cingulate cortex in freshmen enrolled in a medical college during cognitive inhibition on the Stroop task finding no relationship between the course grades and activation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Similarly, there are other studies which suggest a link between inhibitory control and academic performance associating the activation of anterior cingulate with cognitive control across tasks (e.g [ 31 , 34 ]).
The aim of the study described in this paper was to find out whether there is any relationship between adult students’ inhibitory control measured with the Stroop Test and their academic achievement. To measure the degree of inhibitory control, a computerized version of the Stroop Test was used.
Based on the previous research (e.g. [ 29 , 40 , 55 , 57 , 74 ] the initial hypothesis was that there might be a relationship between inhibitory control and academic performance. In this respect, the participants with a higher degree of inhibitory control (lower Stroop Effect) were expected to be those with a higher grade average and lower failure rate than those indicating a lower degree of inhibitory control (higher Stroop Effect). On the other hand, if the correlation between the two variables were to be found, it was not expected to be significantly high since the previous research studying this phenomenon in relation to age points to the inhibitory control exerting its influence on academic performance chiefly at an individual’s young age [ 1 , 4 , 9 , 19 , 28 , 76 ].
107 students studying at (undisclosed) University, Stockholm, Sweden, (29 males, 78 females, mean age = 25.83 years, SD age = 6.32 years, age range = 19–52 years) participated in the study. Originally, 110 students were involved in the study, but 3 of them were removed as outliers due to the overwhelming majority of their grades being at the extreme ends of the grading scale, i.e. either VGs or Us, and only few Gs (for more information on the grading scale, see the next section). This was done to exclude the students whose extraordinary performance in certain academic subjects might be due to either their extra talents for, or their exceptional motivation to study, these subjects. The participants were recruited from students each of whom was enrolled in one of three teacher education programs, i.e. either primary ( N = 21), secondary ( N = 51), or upper secondary ( N = 35). The reason why the participants were recruited from this group was that most of the courses they study within these programs are somewhat similar in terms of contents. Besides, the students are also assessed in these courses mainly by the same teachers. The original idea was to recruit the highest number of volunteers enrolled in the three programs who were, at the same time, studying the courses given by the department in which the study was conducted. Nevertheless, the final number of the participants was determined by their willingness to participate in the study and it was also restricted by the fact that all of them had to be tested on campus in a computer laboratory within the limited time of the project. The volunteers had no neurological or psychiatric disorders. All the participants signed an informed consent with their participation in the study.
Information about the participants’ age and sex was collected via questionnaires distributed among the participants prior to the execution of the Stroop Test. The students’ university grade averages were computed based on their past course grades and their calculation included a computational model (see below) used in another study [ 30 ] researching the effect of mother tongue proficiency on the students’ academic performance.
The study was conducted in an institution using the grading scale consisting of three grades, i.e. VG, G, and U, a system commonly used in Swedish universities. According to this system, VG represents “passed with distinction,” G denotes “passed,” and U denotes “failed.” To facilitate a statistical analysis, these grades were assigned numerical values of 4, 2, and 0, respectively. This approach mirrors the GPA calculation method, where the highest grade corresponds to 4, the middle one to 2, and the fail grade to 0. Each student’s failure rate, expressed as a percentage, was computed as the ratio of their fail grades (Us) to the total number of grades received. Approximately 30 grades, encompassing both courses and graded modules, were considered for the computation of grade averages and failure rates per student. In instances where a student received multiple fail grades for the same course or module, each of these was included as a distinct grade in the calculation.
To measure the participants’ inhibitory control, a computerized version of the Stroop Test available at https://www.psytoolkit.org/ was used. The task was performed in English since the students represent a relatively uniform group when it comes to their English knowledge, which is at the C1 level of Common European Framework of Reference for languages. Moreover, English represents the language that all the participants have studied in a language instructional setting and thus the color identification rule in the Stroop test had to be followed in the context of their knowledge previously adopted at school. In this respect, the experiment made the participants deploy controlled information processing [ 63 ] through the application of a new cognitive concept requiring the inhibition of the semantic contents they have learnt at school before. This way an attempt was made at inducing the situation activating those cognitive processes that resemble the ones which are in operation in school environments when new concepts are learnt or when adjustments are made to the already acquired knowledge.
The task consisted of two conditions on which the participants were tested: (a) congruent trials, where the names of colors displayed on the screen matched the colors these were displayed in, (b) incongruent trails, where the names of colors displayed on the screen did not match the colors these were displayed in. For each trial type the students were instructed to identify the color of the word as quickly as possible by pressing a corresponding key on their keyboards. The keys the subjects were instructed to press were those that bore the initial letters of the names of the colors the words were displayed in. Therefore, when the word “red”, for instance, got displayed in blue, the students were supposed to press the b key (“b” standing for “blue”). The explicit instruction given to the students was to disregard the meaning of the words and focus solely on the color in which these words were displayed.
There were four colors used in the test (red, yellow, blue and green) and the students were instructed to press the r , y , b and g keys, respectively, to indicate these. Before each of the words was presented in the middle of the screen against the black background (for up to 2 s or until the participant responded), a fixation cross was displayed in the same position for 200 milliseconds for the participant to know where the word would appear. Once the participant made their choice, either a word “correct” or “wrong” popped up for 500 milliseconds depending on whether their choice had been correct or not. The computer script in which the test was run measured the participants’ reaction times in both the congruent and incongruent conditions and counted the errors they made when indicating a wrong color. The Stroop test was run under these conditions twice – once as a practice session with thirty trials, whose purpose was to make sure all the participants understood what they were supposed to do as well as to enable them to practice the key-color associations, and then as the test itself with 120 test trials. Half of the test trials were in the congruent condition and the other half in the incongruent one. The congruent and incongruent conditions were mixed and presented to the subjects randomly.
The main Stroop effect size was calculated for the individual participants according to the following formula using the reaction times recorded for congruent as well as incongruent trials:
The reaction times used in the formula above were collected together with the numbers of incorrect color identifications from files saved on a server once the tests had been completed.
Subsequently, Pearson’s bivariate correlation analysis was conducted on the collected data to find out the correlation coefficients between the participants’ grade averages (as well as failure rates) and their Stroop Effect values. Similar analyses were also done for their reaction times in congruent and incongruent conditions as well as their number of Stroop Test mistakes, i.e. the situations where a color was not correctly identified.
As the study group the analysis was conducted with consisted of three sub-groups of students (each sub-group consisting of students enrolled in one of the three teacher training programs), One-Way ANOVA was used to compare these sub-groups for Stroop Effect, reaction times for congruent trials, reaction times for incongruent trials, and number of color identification errors. Since the sub-groups were of unequal sizes, the test of homogeneity of variances was run on all the variables with the subsequent Tukey HSD post hoc test to identify the significance of the differences between the sub-groups.
The students’ ( N = 107) mean Stroop Effect, mean reaction times (in ms) in congruent and incongruent conditions, the mean number of errors as well as the mean number of times the participants exceeded the time limit are given in Table 1 below. The table also lists students’ grade average and fail rate average as well as standard deviations for each variable.
Pearson’s bivariate correlation analysis shows a very weak negative correlation of low statistical significance, r (107) = − 0.13, p = .20, between the Stroop Effect and participants’ grade averages and basically no correlation between the former and participants’ fail rate, r (107) = 0.04, p = .68, (see Table 2 below). Similarly, there seems to be no statistically significant relationship between the participants’ grade averages and their reaction times in congruent ( r (107) = − 0.06, p = .58) and incongruent ( r (107) = − 0.10, p = .31) conditions as well as no significant relationship between the grade averages and the number of times the participants exceeded the time limit for color identification ( r (107) = − 0.07, p = .46). The only statistically significant relationship detected appears to be the one between the students’ grade averages and the number of mistakes (incorrect color identifications) made during the Stroop Test with a weak negative correlation, r (107) = − 0.21, p = .03. Scatter plots for Stroop Effect values and the number of errors made during the test in relation to the students’ grades are shown in Figs. 1 and 2 below. The diagonal lines in the scatter plots represent identity lines indicating the points in which the values of the variables correlate perfectly while the distances of the data points from these lines represent degrees of correlation; the closer the points are to the line, the stronger the correlation between the variables. Tables 3 and 4 show the comparison of measured variables across the educational programs and related post hoc Tukey HSD, respectively.
Grade average – Stroop Effect scatter plot
Grade average – Stroop Test errors scatter plot
The Tukey HSD post hoc test (see Table 4 below) shows the comparison of the distribution of the values within the sub-groups, where the only statistically significant Stroop Effect mean difference of 74.65 ms ( p = .002) can be found between the primary and upper-secondary programs (the students of the latter indicating a lower mean).
The goal of this study was to find out whether there was some relationship between the inhibitory control of the students studying at (undisclosed) University, Stockholm, Sweden, measured with the Stroop Test and their academic performance. Based on the previous research focusing on links between school performance and executive functions [ 51 , 70 , 75 ], the hypothesis was that the participants with a higher grade average and lower fail rate might tend to manifest a higher degree of inhibitory control indicated with a lower Stroop Effect. Nevertheless, this correlation was expected to be weak in the study group consisting of university students since the other research into the area shows the strongest correlation between the inhibitory control and the academic performance in subjects at their early age [ 8 , 10 , 15 , 42 , 61 ].
The results show that although there is a very weak negative correlation ( r (107) = − 0.13) between the Stroop Effect and participants’ grade averages, which might suggest some effect of the degree of their inhibitory control on their school performance that is in line with the previous research focusing on this phenomenon, this relationship has not turned out to be statistically significant ( p = .20). As regards the students’ failure rates, these turned out to be completely independent of the Stroop Effect values.
As regards the comparison of the distribution of all the observed values within the different sub-groups of students depending on what program they study, the only statistically significant difference was found in Stroop Effect (mean difference of 74.65 ms ( p = .002)) between the primary and upper-secondary programs, with the latter indicating a lower Stroop effect.
Another statistically significant relationship detected was the one between the students’ grade averages and the number of mistakes (incorrect color identifications) made during the Stroop Test with a weak negative correlation ( r (107) = − 0.21, p = .03) indicating that the students performing worse academically (having lower grades) made more mistakes during the test. This relationship may suggest that the degree of conscientiousness the students approach the assigned task of Stroop Test with might be in direct proportion to the degree of conscientiousness they approach their university studies with in general. That is [ 41 ], point out that conscientiousness predicts better performance on the Stroop task in terms of fewer errors and diminished incongruency effects. They even suggest that this personality trait may promote certain attentional processes even as cognitive capacities decline at a later age. Similarly, other studies [ 45 , 67 ], deploying other attention control tasks, also found a relationship between conscientiousness and cognitive performance. Finally, as the myriad of studies shows conscientiousness, defined as dependability and will to achieve, as being in direct relationship with academic performance as well [ 21 , 23 , 24 , 58 , etc.], the results of the study described in this paper might be indicative of the newly found relationship between the number of color identification errors (as a factor reflecting this conscientiousness) and academic performance, albeit this relationship has not been studied before.
One of the reasons why the weak correlation between the students’ Stroop Effect and their school performance shows low statistical significance may be that the grade averages had been calculated from the grades the students obtained for a wide range of school subjects ranging from mathematics, to languages, history, etc. That is, the meta-analysis conducted by Pascual & Robres [ 57 ] shows that the degree to which executive functions affect school performance depends on the subject studied. This phenomenon can be observed, for example, in the relationship between mathematics and the visuo-spatial aspect of working memory. Similar observations have also been made when it comes to other executive functions, which appear to be more related to performance in mathematics than in a language, for instance. Moreover, the meta-analysis points out that most studies identify working memory as a better predictor of school performance than inhibition and that executive functions represent an important predictor of academic performance and future learning problems at an early age. However, the predictive capacity of executive functions in relation to academic performance seems to decrease during secondary education and even more so during university education, which might be the case with the study described in this article. The reason for this phenomenon could be minimal individual differences in executive functioning in certain age groups. Bialystok [ 12 ], for instance, in her study of the Stroop task performance, mentions no differences in Stroop Effect among university undergraduates giving the cognitive performance in this age group being at its peak as a reason for the phenomenon. Likewise, Comalli et al. [ 22 ] demonstrated that older adults and children indicate longer response latencies than young adults. The aforementioned factors are also suspected of being the reasons why no correlation has been found between the students’ Stroop Effect and their failure rates.
The fact that the relationship between the students’ Stroop Effect and school performance shows low statistical significance might also be due to the Stroop Test activating different regions of the brain in different individuals – operations that have been documented as correlating with school grades. That is, Veroude et al. [ 74 ] report a significant main effect of cognitive inhibition being observed in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, but not so much in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). However, they report the activation of ACC for the “incongruent” condition being associated with higher grades. In this respect, they found an association with achievement only in the situations where the Stroop Test activated dorsal ACC, indicating that “involvement of this region can potentially predict differences in education success.” (p. 104). Other studies also show the involvement of both the ACC and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during the Stroop task (e.g [ 47 ]). even though the ACC does not seem to be necessary for cognitive control as patients with damage to this region perform normally on the Stroop Test [ 35 ]. As the study described in this paper did not include functional magnetic resonance imaging, it was not possible to find out in which situations the inhibitory control in the subjects in incongruent conditions resulted from the activation of the ACC and in which situations it resulted from the activation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Hence, it is also impossible to assess the extent to which the activation of the former or the latter for inhibitory control might influence the subjects’ grades. Not distinguishing between these two conditions could thus have been one of the reasons for the weak p-value of the results and thus it might be desirable to differentiate between them in future research.
Finally, as the current study suggests a link between the number of mistakes made during the Stroop Test and the students’ grade averages, the potential of the test to be used to measure the degree of their manifested conscientiousness and the ability to concentrate on an assigned, relatively short, one-off task should be studied further. The results of such further testing might provide clues regarding to what extent these characteristics can be viewed as predictors of academic performance.
Overall, this study has shown that there was a weak correlation of low statistical significance between the participants’ grade averages and the inhibitory control measured with the Stroop Test. It has also shown no relationship between their failure rates and inhibitory control.
These findings suggest that differences in the impact of inhibitory control on cognitive functioning among young adults might be much smaller, if any, than in children or older people. This fact seems to be in line with the findings of previous studies which point out that individual differences in executive functions are greatest while these functions are either under development, i.e. in children, or when they are in decline, i.e. in the elderly.
The study has also revealed that the students with lower grades made more color identification errors than those with higher grades. This phenomenon is worth pursuing in the future since the Stroop Test, or any other test where subjects need to follow a relatively simple rule, might be indicative (via their error rates) of their conscientiousness, a way in which they approach a certain assigned task or a degree of their ability to handle the task. Consequently, these findings can offer educators insights into their students’ specific weaknesses in these domains, empowering them to address these areas through tailored teaching approaches, such as individualized activities.
The data pertinent to this study and used in the analysis are enclosed in a separate file uploaded at the submission of the paper.
Altemeier L, Jones J, Abbott RD, Berninger VW. Executive functions in becoming writing readers and reading writers: note taking and report writing in third and fifth graders. Dev Neuropsychol. 2006;29:161–73.
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Antón E, García YF, Carreiras M, Dunabeitia JA. Does bilingualism shape inhibitory control in the elderly? J Mem Lang. 2016;90:147–60.
Article Google Scholar
Alloway T. Working memory, but not IQ, predicts subsequent learning in children with learning difficulties. Eur J Psychol Assess. 2009;25(2):92–8.
Alloway T, Bibile V, Lau G. Computerized working memory training: can it lead to gains in cognitive skills in students? Comput Hum Behav. 2013;29(3):632–8.
Anderson P. Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychol. 2002;8:71–82.
Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: one decade on. Trends Cogn Sci. 2014;18:177–85.
Barrouillet P, Le´pine R. Working memory and children’s use of retrieval to solve addition problems. J Exp Child Psychol. 2005;91(3):183–204.
Bédard AC, Nichols S, Barbosa JA, Schachar R, Logan GD, Tannock R. The development of selective inhibitory control across the life span. Dev Neuropsychol. 2002;21:93–111.
Berg D. Working memory and arithmetic calculation in children: the contributory roles of processing speed, short-term memory, and reading. J Exp Child Psychol. 2008;99:288–308.
Best JR, Miller PH, Naglieri JA. Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learn Individ Differ. 2011;21:327–36.
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Bialystok E, Martin MM. Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: evidence from the dimensional change card sort task. Dev Sci. 2004;7(3):325–39.
Bialystok E, Martin M, Viswanathan M. Bilingualism across the lifespan: the rise and fall of inhibitory control. Int J Biling. 2005;9(1):103–19.
Bialystok E, Craik FIM, Luk G. Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2008;34(4):859–73.
Blair C, Razza RP. Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Dev. 2007;78:647–63.
Bryce D, Whitebread D, Szucs D. The relationships among executive functions, metacognitive skills, and educational achievement in 5 and 7-year-old children. Metacognition Learn. 2015;10:181–98.
Bull R, Espy K. Working memory, executive functioning, and children’s mathematics. Educ Psychol. 2006;93–123.
Bush G, Frazier JA, Rauch SL, Seidman LJ, Whalen PJ, Jenike MA, Rosen BR, Biederman J. Anterior cingulate cortex dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder revealed by MRI and the counting Stroop. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;45(12):1542–52.
Carter CS, Van Veen V. ACC and conflict detection: an update of theory and data. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2007;7(4):367–79.
Cartwright K. Insights from cognitive neuroscience: the importance of executive function for early reading development and education. Early Educ Dev. 2012;23(1):24–36.
Chambers CD, Garavan H, Bellgrove MA. Insights into the neural basis of response inhibition from cognitive and clinical neuroscience. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2009;33:631–46.
Chamorro-Premuzic T. Creativity versus conscientiousness: which is a better predictor of student performance? Appl Cogn Psychol. 2006;20(4):521–31.
Comalli PE Jr, Wapner S, Werner H. Interference effects of Stroop color-word test in childhood, adulthood, and aging. J Genet Psychol. 1962;100:47–53.
Conrad N, Patry MW. Conscientiousness and academic performance: a mediational analysis. Int J Scholarsh Teach Learn. 2012;6(1).
Costa PT Jr. Revised NEO personality inventory and NEO five-factor inventory. Prof Man.
Crede M, Roch SG, Kieszczynka UM. Class attendance in college: a meta-analytic review of the relationship of class attendance with grades and student characteristics. Rev Educ Res. 2010;80(2):272–95.
De Beni R, Palladino P, Pazzaglia F, Cornoldi C. Increases in intrusion errors and working memory deficit of poor comprehenders. Q J Exp Psychol. 1998;51:305–20.
Diamond A. Development of the ability to use recall to guide action, as indicated by infants’ performance on AB. Child Dev. 1985;56:868–83.
Diamond A. Activities and programs that improve children’s executive functions. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2012;21(5):335–41.
Duckworth AL, Taxer JL, Eskreis-Winkler L, Galla BM, Gross JJ. Self-control and academic achievement. Annu Rev Psychol. 2019;70:373–99.
Dvorak M. The varying relationship between perceived oral and written mother tongue proficiency and academic performance in native multilingual students at their secondary school and university. In: 13th International Conference The Future of Education: 2023; Bologna, Italy: Bologna: Pixel International Conferences; 2023;24–27.
Egner T, Etkin A, Gale S, Hirsch J. Dissociable neural systems resolve conflict from emotional versus nonemotional distractors. Cereb Cortex. 2007;18:1475–84.
Eriksen BA, Eriksen CW. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Attent Percept Psychophys. 1974;16:143–9.
Espy KA, McDiarmid MM, Cwik MF, Stalets MM, Hamby A, Senn TE. The contribution of executive functions to emergent mathematical skills in preschool children. Dev Neuropsychol. 2004;26:465–86.
Evers EA, Van der Veen FM, Van Deursen JA, Schmitt JA, Deutz NEP, Jolles J. The effect of acute tryptophan depletion on the BOLD response during performance monitoring and response inhibition in healthy male volunteers. Psychopharmacology. 2006;187:200–8.
Fellows LK, Farah MJ. Is anterior cingulate cortex necessary for cognitive control? Brain. 2005;128(4):788–96.
Friedman NP, Miyake A. The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: a latent-variable analysis. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2004;133:101–35.
Gathercole S, Alloway T, Willis C, Adams A. Working memory in children with reading disabilities. J Exp Child Psychol. 2006;93(3):265–81.
Gerst EH, Cirino PT, Fletcher JM, Yoshida H. Cognitive and behavioral rating measures of executive function as predictors of academic outcomes in children. Child Neuropsychol. 2017;23:381–407.
Glucksberg S, Newsome MR, Goldvarg Y. Inhibition of the literal: filtering Metaphor-Irrelevant Information during Metaphor Comprehension. Metaphor Symbol. 2001;16(3–4):277–98.
Hirsh JB, Inzlicht M. Error-related negativity predicts academic performance. Psychophysiology. 2010;47:192–6.
Huff MJ, Gretz MR, Keefer LA. Conscientiousness predicts performance on the Stroop task but not other attentional control tasks in older and younger adults. Imagin Cogn Pers. 2023;43(2):150–68.
Huizinga M, van der Molen MW. Age-group differences in set-switching and set-maintenance on the Wisconsin Card sorting Task. Dev Neuropsychol. 2007;31:193–215.
Imbrosciano A, Berlach RG. The Stroop test and its relationship to academic performance and general behaviour of young students. Teach Dev. 2005;9:1.
Google Scholar
Irvan R, Tsapali M. The role of Inhibitory Control in Achievement in Early Childhood Education. Camb Educational Res E-J. 2020;7:168–90.
Jackson JD, Balota DA. Mind-wandering in younger and older adults: converging evidence from the sustained attention to Response Task and reading for comprehension. Psychol Aging. 2012;27(1):106–19.
Jiménez M. Competencia social: intervención preventiva en la escuela [Social competence: preventive intervention at school]. Infanc Soc. 2000;24:21–48.
Laird AR, McMillan KM, Lancaster JL, Kochunov P, Turkeltaub PE, Pardo JV, Fox PT. A comparison of label-based review and ALE meta-analysis in the Stroop task. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;25:6–21.
Lee K, Lee Pe M, Ang SJ, Stankov L. Do measures of working memory predict academic proficiency better than measures of intelligence? Psychol Sci. 2009;51(4):403–19.
Locascio G, Mahone EM, Eason SH, Cutting LE. Executive dysfunction among children with reading comprehension deficits. J Learn Disabil. 2010;43(5):44.
McKenzie B, Bull R, Gray C. The effects of phonological and visual-spatial interference on children’s arithmetical performance. Educ Child Psychol. 2003;20(3):93–108.
McLean JF, Hitch GJ. Working memory impairments in children with specific arithmetic learning difficulties. J Exp Child Psychol. 1999;74:240–60.
Mellanby J, Martin M, O’Doherty J. The ‘gender gap’ in final examination results at Oxford University. Br J Psychol. 2000;91(3):377–90.
Miyake A, Friedman NP. The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: four general conclusions. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2012;21(1):8–14.
Miyake A, Shah P. Models of working memory: mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge: University; 1999.
Book Google Scholar
Passolunghi MC, Lanfranchi S. Domain-specific and domain-general precursors of mathematical achievement: a longitudinal study from kindergarten to first grade. Br J Educ Psychol. 2012;82:42–63.
Passolunghi MC, Cornoldi C, De Liberto S. Working memory and intrusions of irrelevant information in a group of specific poor problem solvers. Mem Cogn. 1999;27:779–90.
Pascual CA, Muñoz MN, Robres QA. The relationship between executive functions and academic performance in primary education: review and meta-analysis. Front Psychol. 2019;10:1582.
Poropat AE. A meta-analysis of the five-factor model of personality and academic performance. Psychol Bull. 2009;135(2):322–38.
Privitera AJ, Zhou Y, Xie X. Inhibitory control as a significant predictor of academic performance in Chinese high schoolers. Child Neuropsychol. 2023;29(3):457–73.
Robbins SB, Lauver K, Le H, Davis D, Langley R, Carlstrom A. Do psychosocial and study skill factors predict college outcomes? A meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 2004;130(2):261–88.
Senn TE, Espy KA, Kaufmann PM. Using path analysis to understand executive function organization in preschool children. Dev Neuropsychol. 2004;26:445–64.
St Clair-Thompson HL, Gathercole SE. Executive functions and achievements in school: shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. Q J Exp Psychol. 2006;59:745–59.
Schneider W, Shiffrin RM. Controlled and Automatic Human Information Processing: I. Detection, Search, and attention. Psychol Rev. 1977;84(1).
Schuchardt K, Mähler C, Hasselhorn M. Working memory deficits in children with specific learning disorders. J Learn Disabil. 2008;41(6):514–23.
Simon JR. Reactions toward the source of stimulation. J Exp Psychol. 1969;81:174–6.
Sirin SR. Socioeconomic status and academic achievement: a meta-analytic review of research. Rev Educ Res. 2005;75(3):417–53.
Soubelet A. Age-cognition relations and the personality trait of conscientiousness. J Res Pers. 2011;45(6):529–34.
Stahl C, Voss A, Schmitz F, Nuszbaum M, Tuscher O, Lieb K, et al. Behavioral components of impulsivity. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2014;143:850–86.
Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol. 1935;18:643–62.
Swanson HL. Reading comprehension and working memory in skilled readers: is the phonological loop more important than the executive system? J Exp Child Psychol. 1999;72:1–31.
Tiego J, Testa R, Bellgrove MA, Pantelis C, Whittle S. A hierarchical model of inhibitory control. Front Psychol. 2018;9:1339.
Titz C, Karbach J. Working memory and executive functions: effects of training on academic achievement. Psychol Res. 2014;78:852–68.
Verbruggen F, Logan GD. Response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008;12:418–24.
Veroude K, Jolles J, Knezevic M, Vos CMP, Croiset G, Krabbendam L. Anterior cingulate activation during cognitive control relates to academic performance in medical students. Trends Neurosci Educ. 2013;2:100–6.
Visu-Petra L, Cheie L, Benga O, Miclea M. Cognitive control goes to school: the impact of executive functions on academic performance. Proc Soc Behav Sci. 2011;11:240–4.
Willoughby MT, Kupersmidt JB, Voegler-Lee ME. Is preschool executive function causally related to academic achievement? Child Neuropsychol. 2012;18(1):79–91.
Yow WQ, Li X. Balanced bilingualism and early age of second language acquisition as the underlying mechanisms of a bilingual executive control advantage: why variations in bilingual experiences matter. Front Psychol. 2015;6:164.
Download references
Open access funding provided by Södertörn University. The research project described in the paper was partially funded by the School of Teacher Education, Södertörn University, Stockholm.
Open access funding provided by Södertörn University.
Authors and affiliations.
Södertörn University, Stockholm, Sweden
Martin Dvorak
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
The sole author, Martin Dvorak, has conducted the collection of data, its analysis and all the other procedures described in this paper.
Correspondence to Martin Dvorak .
Ethics approval and consent to participate.
The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki (as revised in 2000). All the subjects participated in the research voluntarily and signed the informed consent. The need for ethics approval has been deemed unnecessary according to Swedish national regulations (Lag (2003:460) om etikprövning av forskning som avser människor), as no information collected or processed during the analysis represents data stipulated in the law as those requiring ethics approval.
Not applicable.
The authors declare no competing interests.
Publisher’s note.
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ .
Reprints and permissions
Cite this article.
Dvorak, M. Inhibitory control and academic achievement – a study of the relationship between Stroop Effect and university students’ academic performance. BMC Psychol 12 , 498 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-024-01984-3
Download citation
Received : 08 September 2023
Accepted : 05 September 2024
Published : 27 September 2024
DOI : https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-024-01984-3
Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:
Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.
Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative
ISSN: 2050-7283
You are accessing a machine-readable page. In order to be human-readable, please install an RSS reader.
All articles published by MDPI are made immediately available worldwide under an open access license. No special permission is required to reuse all or part of the article published by MDPI, including figures and tables. For articles published under an open access Creative Common CC BY license, any part of the article may be reused without permission provided that the original article is clearly cited. For more information, please refer to https://www.mdpi.com/openaccess .
Feature papers represent the most advanced research with significant potential for high impact in the field. A Feature Paper should be a substantial original Article that involves several techniques or approaches, provides an outlook for future research directions and describes possible research applications.
Feature papers are submitted upon individual invitation or recommendation by the scientific editors and must receive positive feedback from the reviewers.
Editor’s Choice articles are based on recommendations by the scientific editors of MDPI journals from around the world. Editors select a small number of articles recently published in the journal that they believe will be particularly interesting to readers, or important in the respective research area. The aim is to provide a snapshot of some of the most exciting work published in the various research areas of the journal.
Original Submission Date Received: .
Find support for a specific problem in the support section of our website.
Please let us know what you think of our products and services.
Visit our dedicated information section to learn more about MDPI.
Examining the relationship between broadband access, parent technology beliefs, and student academic outcomes.
2. literature review, 3. research questions, 4. the edconnect study, 5. culturally responsive evaluation framework, 6. methodology, study setting and data sources, 7.1. rq 1: how do households’ participation in a free community broadband program relate to their children’s math and ela achievement, 7.2. rq 2: how do parents’ beliefs and use of technology relate to students’ math and ela achievement, controlling for household participation in a free community broadband program, 7.3. rq 3: how do students’ personal and household characteristics relate to their math and ela achievement, given their parents’ beliefs and use of technology, and their households participation in a free community broadband program, 8. discussion, 8.1. program participation, 8.2. parents’ beliefs and use of technology, 8.3. racialized/ethnic identity and systemic inequality, 9. conclusions and limitations, author contributions, institutional review board statement, informed consent statement, data availability statement, conflicts of interest.
Click here to enlarge figure
During the Past Month, about How Often Have You Used Technology in Your Home for the Following: | Never | Once or Twice | Several Times |
---|---|---|---|
Accessing information about your child’s grades or performance in school. | 16% | 14% | 71% |
Obtaining information about your child’s homework or assignments. | 21% | 16% | 63% |
Communicating with your child’s teacher or school. | 18% | 20% | 61% |
Obtaining information about a school event or schedule. | 18% | 17% | 65% |
Mean | Std. Dev. | Min | Max | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Parent Use | 0.05 | 0.89 | −2.00 | 0.71 |
Developmental Impact | 0.06 | 0.95 | −2.63 | 1.91 |
Technological Utility | −0.03 | 0.91 | −3.82 | 1.33 |
Familial Challenges | −0.006 | 0.96 | −3.0 | 1.60 |
Variable Name | N | % | |
---|---|---|---|
Econ disadvantaged | |||
Yes | 272 | 53% | |
No | 239 | 47% | |
Total | 511 | 100% | |
Student gender | |||
Female | 255 | 50% | |
Male | 256 | 50% | |
Total | 511 | 100% | |
ELL | |||
Yes | 35 | 7% | |
No | 476 | 93% | |
Total | 511 | 100% | |
Student grade level | |||
3 | 36 | 7% | |
4 | 26 | 5% | |
5 | 98 | 19% | |
6 | 121 | 24% | |
7 | 127 | 25% | |
8 | 103 | 20% | |
Total | 511 | 100% |
Student Racialized/Ethnic Identity | Non-Participating | Participating | Total |
---|---|---|---|
Black (48%) | 117 | 126 | 243 |
Hispanic (14%) | 37 | 32 | 69 |
White (35%) | 138 | 42 | 180 |
Other BIPOC (4%) | 12 | 7 | 19 |
Total (100%) | 304 (59%) | 207 (41%) | 511 (100%) |
ELA | Math | Par Use | Dev Imp | Tech Uti | Fam Chal | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
ELA | 1.00 | |||||
Math | 0.66 | 1.00 | ||||
Par Use | 0.04 | 0.03 | 1.00 | |||
Dev Imp | −0.16 | −0.12 | 0.10 | 1.00 | ||
Tech Uti | 0.06 | −0.02 | 0.33 | −0.06 | 1.00 | |
Fam Chal | −0.14 | −0.17 | −0.05 | 0.04 | −0.01 | 1.00 |
| ||||||||
Household Participation | −7.69 | 3.91 | −1.97 | 0.05 * | −15.37 | −0.02 | 3.88 (1, 486) | 0.006 |
Constant | 309.62 | 2.52 | 122.84 | <0.001 *** | 304.67 | 314.57 | ||
| ||||||||
Household Participation | −9.75 | 3.60 | −2.71 | 0.007 ** | −16.82 | −2.68 | 7.33 (1, 501) | 0.0125 |
Constant | 318.39 | 2.28 | 139.27 | <0.001 *** | 313.90 | 322.88 |
| ||||||||
Household Participation | −6.94 | 3.90 | −1.79 | 0.075 | −14.59 | 0.70 | 8.14 (3, 484) | 0.042 |
Fam Chal | −7.22 | 1.92 | −3.75 | <0.001 *** | −11.00 | −3.44 | ||
Dev Imp | −4.76 | 2.02 | −2.35 | 0.019 ** | −8.73 | −0.78 | ||
Constant | 309.17 | 2.18 | 124.63 | <0.001 *** | 304.30 | 314.04 | ||
| ||||||||
Household Participation | −8.37 | 3.59 | −2.33 | 0.02 ** | −15.42 | −1.33 | 9.54 (3, 499) | 0.049 |
Fam Chal | −5.54 | 1.78 | −3.11 | 0.002 ** | −9.04 | −2.05 | ||
Dev Imp | −6.00 | 1.86 | −3.20 | 0.001 ** | −9.60 | −2.30 | ||
Constant | 317.88 | 2.25 | 141.21 | <0.001 *** | 313.45 | 322.30 |
| ||||||||
Dummy_B | −27.5 | 3.81 | −7.21 | <0.001 *** | −34.94 | −19.98 | 23.83 (5, 482) | 0.19 |
Econ dis | −8.31 | 3.67 | −2.26 | 0.024 * | −15.51 | −1.10 | ||
Grade | −5.71 | 1.23 | −4.65 | <0.001 *** | −8.13 | −3.30 | ||
ELL | −20.39 | 6.91 | −2.95 | 0.003 ** | −34.0 | −6.81 | ||
Fam Chal | −4.64 | 1.79 | −2.59 | 0.01 * | −8.16 | −1.12 | ||
Constant | 359.42 | 7.93 | 45.34 | <0.001 *** | 343.85 | 375.0 | ||
| ||||||||
Econ Dis | −9.93 | 3.34 | −2.96 | 0.003 ** | −16.52 | −3.34 | 20.12 (8, 494) | 0.23 |
ELL | −18.35 | 6.31 | −2.91 | 0.004 ** | −30.75 | −5.94 | ||
Dev Imp | −3.73 | 1.69 | −2.21 | 0.027 * | −7.05 | −0.42 | ||
Tech Uti | 4.86 | 1.83 | 2.65 | 0.008 ** | 1.26 | 8.45 | ||
Fam Chal | −3.20 | 1.63 | −1.96 | 0.05 * | −6.40 | 0.002 | ||
Grade | −2.03 | 1.09 | −1.86 | 0.063 | −4.18 | 0.11 | ||
Gender | −13.13 | 3.15 | −4.16 | <0.001 *** | 19.32 | −6.94 | ||
Dummy_B | −30.69 | 3.54 | −8.67 | <0.001 *** | −27.64 | −23.74 | ||
Constant | 367.20 | 8.78 | 41.80 | <0.001 *** | 349.94 | 384.46 |
The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
Xin, Z.; Bebell, D.; Cleveland, G. Examining the Relationship between Broadband Access, Parent Technology Beliefs, and Student Academic Outcomes. Educ. Sci. 2024 , 14 , 1057. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci14101057
Xin Z, Bebell D, Cleveland G. Examining the Relationship between Broadband Access, Parent Technology Beliefs, and Student Academic Outcomes. Education Sciences . 2024; 14(10):1057. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci14101057
Xin, Zhexun, Damian Bebell, and Gareth Cleveland. 2024. "Examining the Relationship between Broadband Access, Parent Technology Beliefs, and Student Academic Outcomes" Education Sciences 14, no. 10: 1057. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci14101057
Article access statistics, further information, mdpi initiatives, follow mdpi.
Subscribe to receive issue release notifications and newsletters from MDPI journals
42 Accesses
Explore all metrics
Parental educational anxiety is a result of the fierce competition in Chinese education. When their children face a series of academic problems, parents inevitably feel anxious about their children's educational and future development. Therefore, the aim of this study was to investigate the effects of academic stress on parental educational anxiety and the mediating effects of learning anxiety and learning weariness in junior high school students. A total of 467 students from two junior high schools in China and one of their parents (934 participants in total) were invited to complete a questionnaire. SEM analysis revealed that academic stress had a direct ( β = 0.25, p < 0.001) and indirect relationship with educational anxiety through the mediators of learning anxiety ( β = 0.16, p = 0.001) and learning weariness ( β = 0.04, p = 0.039). However, the chain mediating role of learning anxiety and learning weariness in the relationship between academic stress and educational anxiety was not significant ( β = 0.01, p = 0.284). Moreover, the academic stress, learning anxiety and learning weariness of junior high school students were associated with parental educational anxiety, but there was no necessary link between learning anxiety and learning weariness. Regular assessment of the academic stress and academic problems faced by junior high school students and the development of effective interventions are important for alleviating parental educational anxiety. Teachers should pay more attention to students' academic stress rather than focusing only on their grades and provide students with relevant education and assistance to alleviate their learning anxiety, reduce their learning weariness, and prevent or alleviate educational anxiety in parents.
This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.
Subscribe and save.
Price includes VAT (Russian Federation)
Instant access to the full article PDF.
Rent this article via DeepDyve
Institutional subscriptions
Data availability.
The data analysed in the current study are available at the Open Science Framework ( https://osf.io/gnq67/?view_only=2c840bc30c2a4de6a4340e20ba1f9ffb ).
Bai, Y., Liu, X., Zhang, B., Fu, M., Huang, N., Hu, Q., & Guo, J. (2022). Associations of youth mental health, parental psychological distress, and family relationships during the COVID-19 outbreak in China. BMC Psychiatry , 22 . https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-022-03938-8
Baker, T. B., Piper, M. E., McCarthy, D. E., Majeskie, M. R., & Fiore, M. C. (2004). Addiction motivation reformulated: An affective processing model of negative reinforcement. Psychological Review, 111 (1), 33. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.111.1.33
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2009). Beyond diathesis stress: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin, 135 (6), 885. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017376
Bhujade, V. M. (2017). Depression, anxiety and academic stress among college students: A brief review. Indian Journal of Health & Wellbeing, 8 (7), 748–751.
Google Scholar
Bowen, M. (1993). Family therapy in clinical practice . Jason Aronson.
Chen, Y., Huang, R., Lu, Y., & Zhang, K. (2021). Education fever in China: Children’s academic performance and parents’ life satisfaction. Journal of Happiness Studies, 22 (2), 927–954. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-020-00258-0
Article Google Scholar
Chen, G., Oubibi, M., Liang, A., & Zhou, Y. (2022). Parents’ educational anxiety under the “double reduction” policy based on the family and students’ personal factors. Psychology Research and Behavior Management, 15 , 2067–2082. https://doi.org/10.2147/PRBM.S370339
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Cheng, X. Y., & Fu, M. H. (2021). The relationship between parental educational anxiety and elementary school students’ academic emotions: The mediating role of parental educational involvement. Mental Health Education in Primary and Secondary School, 20 , 9–14.
Coyne, J. C., & Downey, G. (1991). Social factors and psychopathology: Stress, social support, and coping processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 42 (1), 401. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ps.42.020191.002153
Crosnoe, R., & Benner, A. D. (2015). Children at school. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science (7th ed., Vol. 4, pp. 268–304). Wiley.
Fu, A., Nie, J., Li, Y., Jin, B., & Cui, J. (2002). A correlation research on interventions in middle school students’ hatred for schooling and learning efficiency. Journal of Psychological Science, 25 , 22–23. https://doi.org/10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.2002.01.008
Gao, X. (2023). Academic stress and academic burnout in adolescents: A moderated mediating model. Frontiers in Psychology, 14 , 1133706. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1133706
Gibbons, I. R. (2022). Parent anxiety, parental psychological control, and adolescent anxiety: Mediation and bidirectional relationships (Doctoral dissertation, Brigham Young University).
Gonzálvez, C., Inglés, C. J., Fernández-Sogorb, A., Sanmartín, R., Vicent, M., & García-Fernández, J. M. (2020). Profiles derived from the school refusal assessment scale-revised and its relationship to anxiety. Educational Psychology, 40 (6), 767–780. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2018.1530734
Hafeez, M., Saira, S., & Ijaz, A. (2022). Relationship between parental anxiety and students’ academic stress at secondary level. International Journal of Learning and Teaching, 14 (1), 12–28. https://doi.org/10.18844/ijlt.v14i1.6271
Hao, Z., Jin, L., Huang, J., & Wu, H. (2022). Stress, academic burnout, smartphone use types and problematic smartphone use: The moderation effects of resilience. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 150 , 324–331. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2022.03.019
Hooda, M., & Saini, A. (2017). Academic anxiety: An overview. Educational Quest, 8 (3), 807–810. https://doi.org/10.5958/2230-7311.2017.00139.8
Huang, J., Cao, M., Zhu, D., & You, X. (2018). Contagion of academic anxiety among intimate senior high school students. Journal of Psychological Science , (6), 1382. https://doi.org/10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.20180614
Iida, M., Gleason, M., Green-Rapaport, A. S., Bolger, N., & Shrout, P. E. (2017). The influence of daily coping on anxiety under examination stress: A model of interindividual differences in intraindividual change. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43 (7), 907–923. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167217700605
Jin, N. N. (2015). Review of domestic research on education anxiety. Journal of Beijing Institute of Education , (03), 31–35. https://doi.org/10.16398/j.cnki.jbjieissn1008-228x.2015.03.007
Jongerden, L., Simon, E., Bodden, D. H. M., Dirksen, C. D., & Bögels, S. M. (2015). Factors associated with the referral of anxious children to mental health care: The influence of family functioning, parenting, parental anxiety and child impairment. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 24 (1), 46–57. https://doi.org/10.1002/mpr.1457
Kim, Y., Kwak, K., & Lee, S. (2016). Does optimism moderate parental achievement pressure and academic stress in Korean children? Current Psychology, 35 (1), 39–43. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-015-9355-5
Li, C., Zhang, X., & Cheng, X. (2022). Associations among academic stress, anxiety, extracurricular participation, and aggression: An examination of the general strain theory in a sample of Chinese adolescents. Current Psychology: A Journal for Diverse Perspectives on Diverse Psychological Issues . https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-022-03204-w
Little, T. D., Cunningham, W. A., Shahar, G., & Widaman, K. F. (2002). To parcel or not to parcel: Exploring the question, weighing the merits. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 9 , 151–173. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_1
Liu, Q., Hong, X., & Wang, M. (2022). Parental educational anxiety during children’s transition to primary school in China. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19 (23), 15479. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192315479
Liu, Z., Xie, Y., Sun, Z., Liu, D., Yin, H., & Shi, L. (2023). Factors associated with academic burnout and its prevalence among university students: A cross-sectional study. BMC Medical Education , 23 (1). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12909-023-04316-y
Luo, X., He, H., & Pan, Y. (2021). Relationship between study weariness, self-compassion and problem behaviors of left-behind adolescents. Chinese Journal of School Health , (07), 1059–1063. https://doi.org/10.16835/j.cnki.1000-9817.2021.07.023
Mikolajczak, M., Raes, M. E., Avalosse, H., & Roskam, I. (2022). Exhausted parents: Sociodemographic, child-related, parent-related, parenting and family-functioning correlates of parental burnout. In Key Topics in Parenting and Behaviour (pp. 57–69). Cham: Springer Nature Switzerland. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-017-0892-4
Minamitani, N., & Matsumoto, Y. (2018). Developmental trial of a cognitive behaviour therapy program for parents of junior high students exhibiting school refusal: Evidence based on a small sample from a metropolitan area in Japan. School Health, 14 , 1–11. https://doi.org/10.20812/jash.SH_086
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2017). Mplus user’s guide (8th ed.). Chapman and Hall/CRC.
Nasser-Abu Alhija, F., & Wisenbaker, J. (2006). A monte carlo study investigating the impact of item parceling strategies on parameter estimates and their standard errors in CFA. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 13 (2), 204–228. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15328007sem1302_3
Núñez-Regueiro, F., & Núñez-Regueiro, S. (2021). Identifying salient stressors of adolescence: A systematic review and content analysis. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 50 (12), 2533–2556. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-021-01492-2
Peng, X., Cai, T., Gui, T., & Fu, J. (2021). Moderating effect of psychological Sushi on relationship between study stress and suicidal ideation in adolescents. Journal of Psychological Science, 11 , 919–924.
Pinquart, M. (2019). Meta-analysis of anxiety in parents of young people with chronic health conditions. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 44 (8), 959–969. https://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsz024
Poole, K. L., Van Lieshout, R. J., McHolm, A. E., Cunningham, C. E., & Schmidt, L. A. (2018). Trajectories of social anxiety in children: Influence of child cortisol reactivity and parental social anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 46 (6), 1309–1319. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-017-0385-3
Rosenthal, L., Moro, M. R., & Benoit, L. (2020). Migrant parents of adolescents with school refusal: A qualitative study of parental distress and cultural barriers in access to care. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10 , 942. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00942
Silver, A. M., Elliott, L., & Libertus, M. E. (2021). When beliefs matter most: Examining children’s math achievement in the context of parental math anxiety. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 201 , 104992. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104992
Staudt, M. (2014). The needs of parents of youth who are truant: Implications for best practices. Best Practice in Mental Health, 10 (1), 47–53.
Surging news. (2021). CCTV Survey: Children's education anxiety up to 36% of major family difficulties in 2020. Baidu. Retrieved April 27, 2021, from: https://baijiahao.baidu.com/s?id=1698156916458803127&wfr=spider&for=pc
Tian, Q., Deng, S. C., & Guo, J. (2012). The influence of self-determination motivation on test anxiety: Procrastinations as a different mediator. Journal of Psychological Science, 35 (5), 1096. https://doi.org/10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.2012.05.019
Trevethan, M., Jain, A. T., Shatiyaseelan, A., Luebbe, A. M., & Raval, V. V. (2022). A longitudinal examination of the relation between academic stress and anxiety symptoms among adolescents in India: The role of physiological hyperarousal and social acceptance. International Journal of Psychology, 57 (3), 401–410. https://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12825
Wu, Y., Schulz, L. E., Frank, M. C., & Gweon, H. (2021). Emotion as information in early social learning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 30 (6), 468–475. https://doi.org/10.1177/09637214211040779
Wu, K., Wang, F., Wang, W., & Li, Y. (2022). Parents’ education anxiety and children’s academic burnout: The role of parental burnout and family function. Frontiers in Psychology, 12 , 764824. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.764824
Xu, J., Cao, J., Cui, L., & Zhu, P. (2010). Preliminary compilation of study stress questionnaire for middle school students. Chinese Journal of School Health, 1 , 68–69. https://doi.org/10.16835/j.cnki.1000-9817.2010.01.032
Xu, F., Cui, W., & Lawrence, P. J. (2020). The intergenerational transmission of anxiety in a Chinese population: The mediating effect of parental control. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 29 (6), 1669–1678. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-019-01675-3
Yan, N., & Ansari, A. (2017). Bidirectional relations between intrusive caregiving among parents and teachers and children’s externalizing behaviour problems. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 41 , 13–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2017.05.004
Yu, S., Zheng, J., Xu, Z., & Zhang, T. (2022). The transformation of parents’ perception of education involution under the background of “double reduction” policy: The mediating role of education anxiety and perception of education equity. Frontiers in Psychology, 13 , 800039. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.800039
Zhao, Y. (2019). Establishment and application of junior middle school Students’ learning weariness scale. Journal of Shanghai Educational Research, 10 , 27–30. https://doi.org/10.16194/j.cnki.31-1059/g4.2019.10.006
Zhao, J. X., Zhao, J. X., & Wang, M. F. (2018). The transmission of anxiety from left-behind women to children: Moderated mediating effect. Psychological Development and Education, 34 (6), 86–93. https://doi.org/10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2018.06.11
Zheng, Q., Wen, N., Xu, F., & Zhu, J. H. (2004). Exploration on and modification of structure of mental health test (MHT). Chinese Journal of Applied Psychology, 02 , 3–7.
Download references
The author would like to acknowledge all the participants in the study. We are very grateful to the editor and reviewers for their work as well as their suggestions for this paper.
This study was funded by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (62077034), Taishan Scholar Project of Shandong Province (tsqn202306153), Jinan City School Integration Development Strategy Project (JNSX2023037), and Haiyou Innovation and Research Team on Cyberpsychology. Funds were used to support data collection.
Authors and affiliations.
School of Psychology, Shandong Normal University, 1 Daxue Road, Changqing District, Jinan, Shandong, China
Lei Han, Xinhang Gao & Wentao Ren
Bishan Bashu Secondary School, Xinyan Road, Biquan Street, Bishan District, Chongqing, China
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
Lei Han: Conceptualization, Resources, Supervision, Writing—review & editing.
Xinhang Gao: Investigation, Writing—original draft, Writing—review & editing.
Wentao Ren: Software, Investigation, Writing—review & editing.
Xujie Wang: Software, Investigation, Writing—review & editing.
Correspondence to Xujie Wang or Wentao Ren .
Informed consent.
Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by all individual participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.
The study involving human participants was reviewed and approved by the Ethical Committee of the School of Psychology, Shandong Normal University (SDNU2024053).
The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's note.
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Lei Han is the first authorship.
Xinhang Gao is the second authorship.
Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.
Reprints and permissions
Han, L., Gao, X., Wang, X. et al. The relationship between academic stress and educational anxiety: learning anxiety and learning weariness as mediators. Curr Psychol (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-024-06738-3
Download citation
Accepted : 17 September 2024
Published : 25 September 2024
DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-024-06738-3
Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:
Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.
Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative
COMMENTS
Introduction. Homework assignment is used regularly as an instructional strategy to optimize students' learning and academic achievement (Cooper et al., 2006; Ramdass and Zimmerman, 2011).In general, there seems to be a positive relationship between homework and academic achievement (Trautwein et al., 2006; Núñez et al., 2015b; Fan et al., 2017), although this relationship will vary in ...
A professor of psychology and neuroscience at Duke analyzes dozens of homework studies and concludes that homework can improve students' scores on class tests, but only up to a certain amount. He also discusses the benefits and drawbacks of homework for different grade levels and suggests homework policies for schools and teachers.
We aim to investigate the role of homework in academic achievement, and to determine the optimum homework time by comparing the differences in outcomes between different groupings of homework time. This will be helpful for teachers and parents to better understand the role and utility of homework, and provide theoretical support for teachers to ...
The relationship between homework behaviors and academic achievement is one of the most important questions in homework field, because it is related to the effectiveness of homework (Cooper et al., 2006, 2012; Fan et al., 2017). Most of the previous studies focused on the relationship between homework time and academic achievement.
both within and across design types, there was generally consistent evidence for a positive influence of homework on achievement. Studies that reported sim-ple homework-achievement correlations revealed evidence that a stronger correlation existed (a) in Grades 7-12 than in K-6 and (b) when students rather than parents reported time on ...
HARRIS COOPER is a Professor of Psychology and Director of the Program in Education, Box 90739, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708-0739; e-mail [email protected] His research interests include how academic activities outside the school day (such as homework, after school programs, and summer school) affect the achievement of children and adolescents; he also studies techniques for improving ...
fo und that teachers who collected, corrected, and graded homework fo und a stronger relationship between homework and achievement. When homework was graded or commented on, it raised learning fr om the 50th to the 79th percentile (Walberg et al., 1985). The literature supports the
tionship between homework and achievement (e.g., Farrow et al., 1999), and when the popular magazine Time made homework its cover story in 1999, it cautioned that homework may overtax children and their families (see also Corno, 1996).4 The present review aims to clarify the much-discussed relationship be tween homework and achievement.
A review of research on the effects of homework on academic achievement in the United States from 1987 to 2003. The authors found positive evidence for homework, but also design flaws and limitations in the studies.
This can cause boredom with homework and learning. To lessen their load and make homework more effective, it is important to establish the connection between homework duration and academic achievement. Objectives. To evaluate the relationship between homework time and academic performance among K-12 students. Search Methods
For instance, a meta-analysis conducted by Cooper, Robinson, and Patall (2006) examined 120 studies on homework and found a moderate positive relationship between homework and achievement in ...
achievement. Overall, we found a positive relationship between homework completion and academic achievement within this upper-level college genetics course and provide implications for increasing student motivation. Keywords: academic achievement; college student performance; genetics education; homework; motivation INTRODUCTION
A review of 60 research studies shows that homework has a positive effect on student achievement, but too much or too little can be counter-productive. The study suggests that the optimal amount of homework should vary according to students' developmental level and home circumstances.
Initially, most research into homework focused on the relationship between academic achievement and homework time, thanks to the abundant evidence about this relationship (Cooper et al., 2006;Fan ...
Despite the long history of homework and homework research, the role that homework plays in enhancing student achievement is, at best, only partly understood. In this review, we give an overview of twentieth-century homework research and discuss the reasons why the relationship between homework and achievement remains unclear. We identify the operationalization of homework and achievement and ...
Despite the long history of homework and homework research, the role that homework plays in enhancing student achievement is, at best, only partly understood. In this review, we give an overview of twentieth-century homework research and discuss the reasons why the relationship between homework and achievement remains unclear. We identify the operationalization of homework and achievement and ...
Homework has been a topic of interest in the public, research and educational arenas throughout the last decades. Yet, researchers disagree on the influence of homework on academic achievement and its value as an instructional technique. Similarly, educators, parents and policymakers have debated on the appropriate amount of homework that students should have, if any. This report reviews the ...
Homework can improve students' scores on class tests, especially in secondary school, but it should not exceed 2.5 hours a night. Learn more about the research evidence, the pros and cons of homework, and the recommendations for parents and educators.
In the present review, we will explore whether the relationship between homework time and academic performance is affected by the mode of homework. Type of homework. Teachers typically assign different kinds of homework according to their purpose. Such as reading story to parents, writing math exercises, and trying scientific experiments.
Does homework promote academic achievement? This web page reviews various studies and arguments on the topic, and suggests some guidelines for effective homework. Learn how homework can improve test scores, develop responsible character, and foster independent learning.
While many scholars have investigated the impact that homework has on academic achievement, there is no strong consensus in the literature. Moreover, most studies have done little to correct for the biases caused by omitted variables that likely influence students' choices regarding study time.2 In this paper, I examine two related issues ...
This article reviews studies on the effects of homework on academic achievement in the United States since 1987. It summarizes the findings, design flaws, and suggestions for future research on homework across different grades, subjects, and purposes.
The existence of relationship between executive functions and academic achievement is also supported by other studies most of which investigate this relationship in children of pre-school or early-school age (e.g. [4, 9, 19, 28, 76, etc.]) or those that study it in the context of learning disabilities [3, 37, 49, 64].
This study explores the relationship between community broadband access, parent technology use and beliefs, and student academic outcomes in a Southeastern U.S. school district during and after the COVID-19 pandemic. By applying a quantitative exploratory approach and multiple regression analysis, the research revealed that parents' technology beliefs and use were significantly associated ...
Parental educational anxiety is a result of the fierce competition in Chinese education. When their children face a series of academic problems, parents inevitably feel anxious about their children's educational and future development. Therefore, the aim of this study was to investigate the effects of academic stress on parental educational anxiety and the mediating effects of learning anxiety ...