Advertisement
The Researcher as Experimental Subject: Using Self-Experimentation to Access Experiences, Understand Social Phenomena, and Stimulate Reflexivity
- Regular Article
- Published: 18 January 2015
- Volume 49 , pages 288–308, ( 2015 )
Cite this article
- Kevin Corti 1 ,
- Geetha Reddy 1 ,
- Ellen Choi 2 &
- Alex Gillespie 1
1317 Accesses
8 Altmetric
Explore all metrics
The current article argues that researcher-as-subject self-experimentation can provide valuable insight and systematic knowledge to social psychologists. This approach, the modus operandi of experimental psychology when the field was in its infancy, has been largely eclipsed by an almost exclusive focus on participant-as-subject other-experimentation. Drawing from the non-experimental first-person traditions of autoethnography, participant observation, and phenomenology, we argue that participating as both observer and subject within one’s own social psychological experiment affords researchers at least three potential benefits: (1) access to “social qualia,” that is, the subjective experience of social phenomena; (2) improved mental models of social phenomena, potentially stimulating new research questions; and (3) an enhanced ability to be reflexive about the given experiment. To support our position, we provide first-person self-reflections from researchers who have self-experimented with transformed social interactions involving Milgram’s cyranoid method. We close by offering guidelines on how one might approach self-experimentation, and discuss a variety of first-person perspective ethnographic technologies that can be incorporated into the practice.
This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.
Access this article
Subscribe and save.
- Get 10 units per month
- Download Article/Chapter or eBook
- 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
- Cancel anytime
Price includes VAT (Russian Federation)
Instant access to the full article PDF.
Rent this article via DeepDyve
Institutional subscriptions
Similar content being viewed by others
Breaking from the Field: Participant Observation and Bourdieu’s Participant Objectivation
Participant Observation: The Personal Commitment in Native Life—A Problematic Methodological Topos
Observational Methods in Sexuality Research
Adair, J. G. (1991). Social cognition, artifact, and the passing of the so-called crisis in social psychology. Canadian Psychology, 32 (3), 445–450.
Article Google Scholar
Anderson, L. (2006). Analytic autoethnography. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography, 35 (4), 373–395.
Becker, H. S., & Geer, B. (1957). Participant observation and interviewing: a comparison. Human Organization, 16 (3), 28–32.
Berger, P. L., & Luckmann, T. (1967). The social construction of reality . New York: Doubleday.
Blass, T. (2004). The man who shocked the world: The life and legacy of Stanley Milgram . New York: Basic Books.
Google Scholar
Boellstorff, T. (2008). Coming of age in second life: An anthropologist explores the virtually human . Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Boring, E. G. (1915). The sensations of the alimentary canal. American Journal of Psychology, 26 (1), 1–57.
Cabanac, M. (2004). Dionysians and Apollonians [Peer commentary on “self-experimentation as a source of new ideas: ten examples about sleep, mood, health, and weight”, by S. Roberts]. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27 (2), 263–264.
Cantor, N., & Mischel, W. (1979). Prototypes in person perception. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 12 , 3–52.
Carpenter, R. (2014). Cleverbot . Retrieved from http://www.cleverbot.com .
Cornish, F., Zittoun, T., & Gillespie, A. (2007). A cultural psychological reflection on collaborative research. Forum: Qualitative Social Research, 8 (3). Retrieved from http://www.qualitative-research.net/index.php/fqs/article/view/309/677 .
Corti, K., & Gillespie, A. (2015). Revisiting Milgram’s cyranoid method: experimenting with hybrid human agents. Journal of Social Psychology, 155 (1), 30–56.
Chang, H. (2008). Autoethnography as method . Walnut Creek: Left Coast Press.
Danziger, K. (1979). The positivist repudiation of Wundt. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 15 (3), 205–230.
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Danziger, K. (1980). Wundt’s psychological experiment in the light of his philosophy of science. Psychological Researcher, 42 , 109–122.
Danziger, K. (1985). The origins of the psychological experiment as a social institution. American Psychologist, 40 (2), 133–140.
Danziger, K. (1990). Constructing the subject: Historical origins of psychological research . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Book Google Scholar
Danziger, K. (2000). Making social psychology experimental: a conceptual history, 1920–1970. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 36 (4), 329–347.
Dennett, D. C. (2007). Heterophenomenology reconsidered. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 6 (1), 247–270.
Denzin, N. K., & Lincoln, Y. (2000). Handbook of qualitative research . Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.
Doherty, A. R., Caprani, N., Conaire, C. Ó., Kalnikaite, V., Gurrin, C., Smeaton, A. F., & O’Connor, N. E. (2011). Passively recognising human activities through lifelogging. Computers in Human Behavior, 27 (5), 1948–1958.
Dumitrica, D., & Gaden, G. (2009). Knee-high boots and six-pack abs: auto ethnographic reflections on gender and technology in second life. Journal of Virtual Worlds Research, 1 (3), 3–23.
Elms, A. C. (1975). The crisis of confidence in social psychology. American Psychologist, 30 (10), 967–976.
Farr, R. M. (1978). On the social significance of artifacts in experimenting. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 17 (4), 299–306.
Farr, R. M. (1983). Wilhelm Wundt (1832–1920) and the origins of psychology as an experimental and social science. British Journal of Social Psychology, 22 (4), 289–301.
Farr, R. M. (1996). The roots of modern social psychology . Oxford: Blackwell.
Fleck, R., & Fitzpatrick, G. (2009). Teachers’ and tutors’ social reflection around SenseCam images. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 67 (12), 1024–1036.
Gallagher, S., & Sørensen, J. B. (2006). Experimenting with phenomenology. Consciousness and Cognition, 15 (1), 119–134.
Galton, F. (1879). Psychometric experiments. Brain, 2 (2), 149–162.
Garfinkel, H. (1967). Studies in ethnomethodology . Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.
Gillespie, A., & Martin, J. (2014). Position exchange theory: a socio-material basis for discursive and psychological positioning. New Ideas in Psychology, 32 , 73–79.
Gillespie, A., & Zittoun, T. (2010). Studying the movement of thought. In A. Toomela & J. Valsiner (Eds.), Methodological thinking in psychology (pp. 69–88). Charlotte: Information Age Publishing.
Giorgi, A. (1998). The origins of the Journal of Phenomenological Psychology and some difficulties in introducing phenomenology into scientific psychology. Journal of Phenomenological Psychology, 29 (2), 161–176.
Glăveanu, V. P., & Lahlou, S. (2012). Through the creator’s eyes: using the subjective camera to study craft creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 24 (2–3), 152–162.
Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life . London: Penguin.
Gottschalk, S. (2010). The presentation of avatars in second life: self and interaction in social virtual spaces. Symbolic Interaction, 33 (4), 501–525.
Gurrin, C., Smeaton, A. F., Byrne, D., O’Hare, N., Jones, G. J. F., & O’Conner, N. (2008). Information retrieval technology (4993rd ed., pp. 537–542). Berlin: Springer. Lecture notes in computer science.
Habermas, J. (1968). Knowledge and human interests . Cambridge: Polity Press.
Ikegami, E. (2011). Visualizing the networked self: agency, reflexivity, and the social life of avatars. Social Research, 78 (4), 1155–1184.
Jackson, F. (1982). Epiphenomenal qualia. The Philosophical Quarterly, 32 (127), 127–136.
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models: Towards a cognitive science of language, inference, and consciousness . Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Klimoski, R., & Mohammed, S. (1994). Team mental model: construct or metaphor? Journal of Management, 20 (2), 403–437.
Klein, P., & Westcott, M. R. (1994). The changing character of phenomenological psychology. Canadian Psychology, 35 (2), 133–158.
Kusch, M. (1995). Recluse, interlocutor, interrogator: natural and social order in turn-of-the-century psychological research schools. Isis, 86 (3), 419–439.
Lahlou, S. (2010). Digitization and transmission of human experience. Social Science Information, 49 (3), 291–327.
Lahlou, S. (2011). How can we capture the subject’s perspective? An evidence-based approach for the social scientist. Social Science Information, 50 (3–4), 607–655.
Lahlou, S., Le Bellu, S., & Boesen-Mariani, S. (2015). Subjective evidence based ethnography: method and applications. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science . doi: 10.1007/s12124-014-9288-9 . Advance online publication.
PubMed Google Scholar
Lafferty, J. T. (1987). Subarctic survival situation . Plymouth: Human Synergistics.
Lee, V. R. (2013). The quantified self (QS) movement and some emerging opportunities for the educational technology field. Educational Technology, 53 (6), 39–42.
Lubart, T. I., & Mouchiroud, C. (2004). Why does self-experimentation lead to creative ideas? [Peer commentary on “Self-experimentation as a source of new ideas: ten examples about sleep, mood, health, and weight”, by S. Roberts]. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27 (2), 269–270.
Marbach, E. (2007). No heterophenomenology without autophenomenology: variations on a theme of mine. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 6 (1), 75–87.
Marslen-Wilson, W. (1973). Linguistic structure and speech shadowing at very short latencies. Nature, 244 , 522–523.
Mead, G. H. (1932). In A. E. Murphy (Ed.), The philosophy of the present . La Salle: Open Court.
Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioral study of obedience. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67 (4), 371–378.
Milgram, S. (2010a). Cyranoids. In T. Blass (Ed.), The individual in a social world: Essays and experiments (3rd ed., pp. 402–409). London: Pinter & Martin.
Milgram, S. (2010b). On maintaining social norms: A field experiment in the subway. In T. Blass (Ed.), The individual in a social world: Essays and experiments (3rd ed., pp. 34–41). London: Pinter & Martin.
Milgram, S. (2010c). Response to intrusion into waiting lines. In T. Blass (Ed.), The individual in a social world: Essays and experiments (3rd ed., pp. 42–55). London: Pinter & Martin.
Mischel, T. (1970). Wundt and the conceptual foundations of psychology. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 31 (1), 1–26.
Nagel, T. (1974). What is it like to be a bat? Philosophical Review, 83 (4), 435–450.
Nakamura, L. (2002). Cybertypes: Race, ethnicity, and identity on the internet . New York: Routledge.
Neuringer, A. (1981). Self-experimentation: a call for change. Behaviorism, 9 (1), 79–94.
Rafalovich, A. (2006). Making sociology relevant: the assignment and application of breaching experiments. Teaching Sociology, 34 (2), 156–163.
Roberts, S. (2004). Self-experimentation as a source of new ideas: ten examples about sleep, mood, health, and weight. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27 (2), 227–262.
Roberts, S. (2012). The reception of my self-experimentation. Journal of Business Research, 65 (7), 1060–1066.
Rosenthal, R. (1966). Experimenter effects in behavioral research . New York: Appleton.
Schumacher, R. M., & Czerwinski, M. P. (1992). Mental models and the acquisition of expert knowledge. In R. R. Hoffman (Ed.), The psychology of expertise . New York: Springer.
Shotter, J. (2015). Undisciplining social science: Wittgenstein and the art of creating situated practices of social inquiry. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour .
Snyder, M., Tanke, E. D., & Berscheid, E. (1977). Social perception and interpersonal behavior: on the self-fulfilling nature of social stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35 (9), 656–666.
Strenski, I. (1982). Malinowski: second positivism, second romanticism. Man, 17 (4), 766–771.
Tajfel, H., Sheikh, A. A., & Gardner, R. C. (1964). Content of stereotypes and the inference of similarity between members of stereotyped groups. Acta Psychologica, 22 , 191–201.
Titchener, E. B. (1927). Experimental psychology: A manual of laboratory of practice (Vol. 1). New York: Macmillan.
Varela, F. J., & Shear, J. (1999). First-person methodologies: what, why, how? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6 (2–3), 1–14.
Vermersch, P. (1999). Introspection as practice. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6 (2–3), 17–42.
Wagoner, B. (2009). The experimental methodology of constructive microgenesis. In J. Valsiner, P. C. M. Molenaar, M. C. D. P. Lyra, & N. Chaudhary (Eds.), Dynamic process methodology in the social and developmental sciences . New York: Springer.
Williams, M. (2007). Avatar watching: participant observation in graphical online environments. Qualitative Research, 7 (1), 5–24.
Wilson, T. D. (2003). Knowing when to ask: introspection and the adaptive unconscious. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 10 (9–10), 131–140.
Winston, A. S., & Blais, D. J. (1996). What counts as an experiment? A transdisciplinary analysis of textbooks, 1930–1970. American Journal of Psychology, 109 (4), 599–616.
Yee, N., & Bailenson, J. (2007). The Proteus effect: the effect of transformed self-representation on behavior. Human Communication Research, 33 (3), 271–290.
Young, G., & Whitty, M. T. (2010). In search of the cartesian self: an examination of disembodiment within 21st-century communication. Theory & Psychology, 20 (2), 209–229.
Zimbardo, P. G. (1992). Foreword. In J. Sabini & M. Silver (Eds.), The individual in a social world: Essays and experiments (2nd ed., pp. ix–xi). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Download references
Author information
Authors and affiliations.
London School of Economics, London, UK
Kevin Corti, Geetha Reddy & Alex Gillespie
Western University, Ontario, Canada
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
Corresponding author
Correspondence to Kevin Corti .
Rights and permissions
Reprints and permissions
About this article
Corti, K., Reddy, G., Choi, E. et al. The Researcher as Experimental Subject: Using Self-Experimentation to Access Experiences, Understand Social Phenomena, and Stimulate Reflexivity. Integr. psych. behav. 49 , 288–308 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-015-9294-6
Download citation
Published : 18 January 2015
Issue Date : June 2015
DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-015-9294-6
Share this article
Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:
Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.
Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative
- First-person methodology
- Phenomenology
- Self-experimentation
- Find a journal
- Publish with us
- Track your research
IMAGES
VIDEO